/
Static Vowels  ► Diphthongs Static Vowels  ► Diphthongs

Static Vowels ► Diphthongs - PowerPoint Presentation

alida-meadow
alida-meadow . @alida-meadow
Follow
374 views
Uploaded On 2018-11-06

Static Vowels ► Diphthongs - PPT Presentation

Semivowels Stops Extending SF Theory to Accommodate Articulatory Movement ɑ i u This shows the formant patterns for three static vowels English has 3 diphthongs ID: 718512

articulatory vowel differences we

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Presentation The PPT/PDF document "Static Vowels ► Diphthongs" is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

Slide1

Static Vowels ►Diphthongs ► Semivowels ► Stops

Extending SF Theory to Accommodate Articulatory MovementSlide2

[ɑ]

[

i

]

[u]

This shows the formant patterns for three

static

vowels. English has 3 diphthongs

(

nonstatic

)

that include these vowel qualities:

[

ai

]

(

buy

),

[au]

(

bow

), and

[

oi

]

(

boy

).

(You may have learned different ways to transcribe these sounds; don’t worry about it.)

What patterns would you expect for

[

ai

], [au],

and

[

oi

]

?Slide3

[

ai

]

[au]

[

oi

]

natural

synthetic

The principle is straightforward: to get

[

ai

],

you start around

[ɑ]

and

transition

into something around

[

i

]

. Same idea with

[au]

and

[

oi

].

These dynamic changes in formants are called

formant transitions

.

They’re a very big deal – for

vowels and for consonants

.Slide4

[

ai

]

[au]

[

ɔ

i

]

natural

synthetic

The synthetic copies here are purposely cartoonish – I connected the formants for the 1st vowel to those of the 2

nd

vowel with straight lines. The ear doesn’t seem to be real fussy about the fine details. Slide5

Ok, let’s review what we’re trying to do here – how do we extend the simple (and very limiting) static SF model we’ve been working with to a dynamic SF model that we need to understand how actual speech works?

At this point we’ve extended a

static vowel model

to a

dynamic vowel model. It may not seem like we’ve gone that far, but we have – all the rest of the steps will look very similar. Slide6

Next steps: Push the dynamic vowel idea to the most vowel-like consonants: semivowels (glides).

Push the semivowel idea to the

least

vowel-like consonants: stops.Slide7

[

]

[

]

[

]Slide8

[iɑ][

j

ɑ

]

[dɑ

]Slide9

[

]

Going left to right, the sound category (manner class) is changing – diphthong

► semivowel

stop.

What is the

articulatory

feature that distinguishes these three manner class categories?

What is the

acoustic feature that distinguishes these three manner class categories?

[

wɑ]

[

bɑ]

[

]

[

]

[

]Slide10

Articulatory difference: speed of articulatory movement – diphthongs: slow movements; semivowels: medium speed; stops: very fast.

Acoustic difference

:

formant transition duration

– diphthongs: long transitions; semivowels: medium- length transitions; stops: very short transitons

.

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]Slide11

One quick point before we move to the next question: These articulatory and acoustic facts have to be connected in this way.

Articulation controls sound

:

if the articulators move slowly, the formants have no choice but to change slowly; if the articulators move quickly, the formants have no choice but to move quickly.

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]Slide12

Comparing top to bottom ([uɑ] vs [

], [

] vs. [jɑ

], etc.

), the sounds differ in the

starting articulatory configuration

.

Top row is simple: They’re all labial sounds.

Bottom row not quite so simple, but they all

start with a high tongue position,

then move to something like

].

For simplicity, we’re going to call the top-bottom difference

place of articulation.

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]Slide13

Ok, we’re going to characterize differences between [u]-[i], [w]-[j],

and

[b

]-[d]

as place of articulation. The terminology is a slight stretch, but conceptually it works fine. Top row:

labials, bottom row: we’ll call these “

alveolar/palatals

for convenience

(the tongue blade/tip is raised)

.

Now the question:

What acoustic information conveys these “place” differences to the listener?

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]Slide14

Two possibilities can be ruled out: (1) F1 rises for both “places”; that can’t be it. (2) F3

slants downward for the “palatals”; that could be it, but it’s not. Just trust me.

What’s left?

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]

[

]Slide15

Bottom line:This particular manner class difference

(diphthongs vs. semivowels vs. stops) is conveyed by

differences in formant transition duration

(due to differences in the speed of articulatory movement).

Differences in place of articulation are conveyed by

differences in the trajectory of F2

transitions.

(

F

3

transitions matter also, but that’s a story we won’t get into.)