/
tion tion

tion - PDF document

alida-meadow
alida-meadow . @alida-meadow
Follow
379 views
Uploaded On 2016-10-05

tion - PPT Presentation

212StimuliFig1showstheexemplarandtwoofthetestobjectsTheexemplarhadabasediameterof6cmandaheightof6cmThreeverticallyextendedtestobjectshadthesamebasebutdifferingheightsof8cmV112cmV2or16cmV3 ID: 471909

2.1.2.StimuliFig.1showstheexemplarandtwoofthetestobjects.Theexemplarhadabasediameterof6cmandaheightof6cm.Threeverticallyextendedtestobjectshadthesamebasebutdifferingheightsof8cm(V1) 12cm(V2) or16cm(V3

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Pdf The PPT/PDF document "tion" is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

tion—ahorizontalsurface(onwhichtosit)properlyalignedwithaverticalsurface(tosupportone’sback).Tosolvethisproblem,theoriesofhumanobjectrecognitionandalgorithmsfortherecognitionofobjectsbymachinesoftenincludearoleforcategorylearning(e.g.,Duvdevani-Bar&Edelman,1999;Ullman,1996).Theimplicationisthatcategory-relevantdescriptionsofobjectshapearenotapriorideterminantsofobjectcategoriesbutratherthedevelopmental product of forming those categories.Recentdevelopmentaldatasupportthisidea.Duringtheperiodfrom1to3yearsofage,childrenacquirenamesforcommonobjectcategories,andtheyalsoshiftfrommorefeature-basedtomoreglobal-shape-basedobjectrecognition(see,forexample,Jones&Smith,2004;Nelson,1972;Quinn,2004;Rakison&Cohen,1999).Inonestudy(Smith,2003),youngchildrenwerepresentedwithrichlydetailedandtypicalinstancesofcommonobjectcategories,orwithminimalistthree-dimensionalrepresentationsofthesesamethings.Theseminimalistrepresentationsweremadefromthreetofourgeometricvolumesalignedintheproperspatialarrangement.Theresultsshoweddramaticdevelopmentalchangelinkedtochildren’sknowledgeofcommonobjectcategories.Two-year-oldswhoknewrelativelymanyobjectcategoriesfortheiragerecognizedtheminimalshaperepresentationsaswellastheydidtherichlydetailedones.Incontrast,same-agedchildrenwhodidnotknowasmanycategoriesdidnotrecognizetheminimalshapesatall,eventhoughtheyrecognizedtherichlydetailedversionsaswellasthemoreadvancedchildren.Thusthereseemstobealinkbetweenlearningobjectcategoriesandlearningaminimalistdescriptionofobjectshape,thekindofdescriptionthatwouldallowfortheimmediaterecognitionofawidevarietyofin-stancesofacategory.Insupportofthissecondidea,asecondexperiment(Smith,2003)showedthatthemoreadvanced2-year-oldswereabletorecognizeminimalistversionsofevennovelobjects.Childrenthusappeartobelearningnotjustthecategory-relevantshapesofspecificthings,buttheyarealsolearningtodescribetheshapeofanythinginacate-gory-relevantway.1.2.Action is also a good candidate for teaching category-relevantAstheoristsfromavarietyofperspectiveshavenoted,thereisacausallinkbetweenshapeandfunction(e.g.,Gelman&Bloom,2000;J.J.Gibson,1966).Itisbecausechairshavehorizontalandverticalsurfacesthatwecansitonthemwithbacksupport,andthustheseatsandbacksofchairsaremoredefiningof“chair-shape”thanthecurveofthelegsorthepresenceofarms.Aroleforactionisalsosuggestedbygrowingevidencethatourhabitualmannerofactingonanobjectispartofthememoryforthatobject,suchthatthevisualrecognitionofathingautomaticallyactivatestheactionsassociatedwithit(seeCreem&Proffitt,2001;Ellis&Tucker,2000;Glenberg,Robertson,Kaschak,&Malter,2003).Otherevidenceindicatesthatobject-relatedactionsandobjectidentificationrelyoncommonbrainregions(e.g.,Faillenot,Toni,Decety,Gregoire,&Jeannerod,1997;Grezes&Decety,2001).Mightnotaction,then,playaformativeroleintherepresentationofanobject’sshapeandperhapsalsoincreatingtheverydimensionsofshapeacrosswhichobjectsarecategorizedandcompared?L. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005) 2.1.2.StimuliFig.1showstheexemplarandtwoofthetestobjects.Theexemplarhadabasediameterof6cmandaheightof6cm.Threeverticallyextendedtestobjectshadthesamebasebutdifferingheightsof8cm(V1),12cm(V2),or16cm(V3).Threehorizontallyextendedtestobjectshadheightsof6cm;thebaseofthesetestobjectsvariedprogressivelyfromacircletoellipsewithmajordiametersof8cm(H1),12cm(H2),and16cm(H3).TheseobjectswerecarvedfromwhiteStyrofoamwithblackprotrusionsonthesidesforaddedinterest.Ameter-widebymeter-tallwoodenbackdropwithgrasspaintedalongthebottomandatreeontherightsidewasusedtoguidetheactions.Forwarm-up,theexperimenterusedatoyflower,bunny,car,and2.1.3.Forced-choice procedureTheforced-choiceprocedurebeganwithawarm-uptasktohelpchildrenunderstandthetesttrials.Thechildwaspresentedwithtwowell-knownobjects,forexample,theflowerandbunny,placedonthetableapproximately20cmapart.Thechildwasasked,“Whereisthebunny?Showmethebunny?”Childrenwerehelpedtomakethisfirstresponseiftheydidnotdosoimmediatelyandwarmfeedbackwasprovidedforallcorrectresponses.Thiswasrepeatedwiththeexperimentervaryingtherequestedobjectuntilthechildrespondedcorrectlyon three consecutive trials. All children did so within eight warm-up trials.Immediatelyafterthewarm-uptrials,themainexperimentbeganwiththeprimingevent.Theexperimenterandchildstoodononesideofatablefacingthebackdrop.TheexperimenterL. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005) Fig. 1.The exemplar (A) and two of the test objects (B) used in Experiment 1. introducedtheexemplarandtoldthechild“Thisisawug.”Inthetwoactionconditions,theexperimenterthentooktheexemplarandmoveditthefulllengthalongthegrass(inthehorizontalcondition)oralongthetree(intheverticalcondition).Thechildwasthenaskedtotakethe“wug”andmoveitinthesamewayastheexperimenter.Inbothactionconditions,thechildheldtheobjectinthesameway,withthepalminaverticalorientationwiththefingerswrappedaroundtheexemplar.Theexperimenterhelpedthechildifnecessaryandmadesurethechildmovedtheobjectthefulllengthbackandforthacrossthepaththreetimes(i.e.,sixlengths).Theexperimenterthenremovedtheexemplarandplacedtwotestobjectsonthetable,askingthe child, “Where is the wug? Show me the wug.” No feedback was provided on test trials.Theproceduresintheno-actionhorizontalandverticalconditionswerethesame,excepttheexperimenterneveraskedthechildtomovetheobject.Aftertheinitialdemonstrationoftheaction,theexperimenteraskedthechildtowatchastheexperimentermovedtheexemplarbackandforththreemoretimes.Intheno-action–no-movementcondition,theexperimentermerelyintroduced the exemplar and set it on the table, naming it several times.Theexemplarwasremovedfromsightaftertheseprimingevents,andaseriesof12testtrials(presentedinoneoftworandomorders)followed.Theseincluded4eachofthreecontrasts:H1versusV1,H2versusV2,andH3versusV3.Halfwaythroughtesting,theexperimenterre-peatedtheprimingeventappropriatetotheconditionandthenremovedtheexemplarbefore2.1.4.Yes–no procedureTheyes–noprocedurealsobeganwithwarm-uptrials.Thechildwaspresentedwithonewarm-upobjectatatimeandthenaskedifitcouldbecalledbyacertainname.Thesetrialswerearrangedsothattherequiredanswercouldbea“yes”ora“no.”Forexample,theexperi-mentermightpresentthechildwiththeappleandask,“Isthisanapple?”(thechildshouldsay“yes”),andonthenexttrial,theexperimentermightpresentthechildwiththebunnyandask,“Isthisacar?”(thechildshouldsay“no”).Childrenweregivenfeedback.Afterachildansweredthreeconsecutivequestionscorrectly,themainexperimentbeganwiththeprimingevent.Theprocedurewasidenticaltothatoftheforced-choiceversionexceptduringthetesttrials.Underthisprocedure,childrenwerequeriedabouteachofthesixtestobjectsindividually,withtheexperimenteraskingofeachtestobject,“Isthisawug?”Nofeedbackwasprovided.Eachtestobjectwasqueriedthreetimesfor18totaltrials.Childrenwererequiredtopassthewarm-upprocedurein10trialsandtoalsoanswer“no”atleasttwiceduringthetestingtrials. Four children failed to meet these criteria and were replaced.Each version of the experiment took approximately 30 min.2.2.Results and discussionFig.2showstheproportionoftimeschildrenchosethehorizontallyextendedtestobjectovertheverticallyextendedoneintheforced-choicetask.Inthetwoactionconditions,childrenchosethehorizontallyextendedtestobjectwhentheyhadmovedtheexemplaralongahorizontalpathbutchosetheverticallyextendedonewhentheyhadmovedtheexemplaralongaverticalpath,asindicatedbyasignificantmaineffectofdirection,(1,22)=53.01,.001.Therewasalsoaninteractionbetweendirectionandtestcontrast,(2,44)=3.44,L. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005) .05.Notsurprisingly,perhaps,inthisforced-choiceprocedurechildrenhadsomedifficultychoosingbetweentwoalternativesthatdifferedlittleintheirhorizontalandverticalextentfromeachotherandthatalsodifferedlittlefromtheexemplar.Still,theeffectofactionisreliableforallcontrasts(Tukeyhonestlysignificantdifferencetest,.05).Children’schoicesinthethreeno-actionconditionsdidnotdifferfromchanceorfromoneanother.Thus,inthetwoconditionsinwhichchildrenwatchedtheexperimentermovetheexemplarhorizontallyorvertically,therewerenosystematicdirectionaleffects.Manyindividualchildrenchoseinadirectionallyconsistentway(i.e.,preferringthehorizontallyorverticallyextendedobjects),buttheirpreferenceswerenotrelatedtocondition.Theforced-choiceprocedureforceschildrentochoosebetweentwoalternatives—bothofwhichorneitherofwhich—mightbeseenasgoodcategorymembers.Thetaskthusprovidesameasureoftherelativeimportanceofverticalversushorizontalextension,andtheresultsshowthattherelativeimportancedependsonthechild’sactions.Theyes–noprocedure,incontrast,assessestheperceivedsimilarityofeachtestobjecttotheexemplarsingly,andthusthechildisnotforcedbytaskstructuretoincludeorexcludeanytestobjectfromthecategory.Fig.3showstheproportionof“yes”responses(thetestobjectisawug)inthistask.Again,onlyintheactionconditionswasthereadirectionalef-fect.Morespecifically,intheactionconditionstherewasaninteractionbetweenthedirec-tionoftheactionandmagnitudeofvertical–horizontalextent,(2,44)=9.48,.001,be-tweenthehorizontal–verticalextensionofthetestobjectandmagnitudeofthatextent,44)=8,72,.001,andbetweendirection,horizontal–verticalextension,andmagnitudeofthatextent,(2,44)=10.32,.001.Thatis,childreninthehorizontalactionconditionsaid“yes”moretothehorizontallythantotheverticallyextendedtestobjects,andchildrenintheverticalconditionsaid“yes”moretotheverticallyextendedthantothehorizontallyextendedtestobjects,andtheseeffectsweregreaterforthetestobjectsofintermediatesimi-laritytotheexemplar(Tukeyhonestlysignificantdifferencetest,.05).Inbrief,childrenaremorelikelytoincludetestobjectsthatdifferbysmallerthanbygreateramountsfromtheexemplarinthecategory,butdirectionalactionbroadensthemagnitudeofallowablewithin-categorydifferencesthatarecongruentwiththedirectionofaction.Intheno-actionconditions,therewasonlyamaineffectofmagnitudeofvertical–horizontalextent,(2,66)=8.97,.001.Thetwotaskproceduresimposeverydifferenttaskdemands(choosingthe“better”oftwoalternativesversusmakingindividualdecisionsabouttheinclusionornoninclusionofanobject).Yetbothindicatethatactionstronglyinfluencestherangeofshapestakenassimilar.Theseeffectsobtainonlyintheconditionsinwhichchildrenactonobjectsandnotintheconditionsinwhichtheywatchtheexperimenterperformthesameactions.Thisfactconstrainspossible explanations as will be considered in the General Discussion.3.Experiment 2Experiment2demonstratesthegeneralityofthephenomenonbyexaminingthecaseofsymmetry.L. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005) 3.1.Method3.1.1.ParticipantsChildren(60girls,60boys)betweentheagesof24and34months,(meanage30months)participated,halfinaforced-choiceversionandhalfinayes–noversion.NonehadparticipatedinExperiment1.Threechildrenfailedtomeetthecriteriaforinclusionintheyes–no3.1.2.Stimuli and procedureAllaspectsofthedesignandprocedurewereidenticaltoExperiment1,withtheexceptionofthestimuliandtheactions.Fig.4showstheexemplarandtestobjects.Theexemplarwas16cmlongatitsbaseand3cmdeepwithtwo“bumps”oneachend,one6.5cmhigh,theother4.5cmhigh,andaheightof2.5cmatthemidpointbetweenthem.Thetestobjectswerearrangedinpairsorderedbytheiroverallsimilaritytotheexemplar:A1versusS1,A2versusS2,andA3versusS3.TheAmemberwaslesssymmetric,andthememberwasmoresymmetricthantheexemplar. All stimuli were carved from Styrofoam and painted purple.Fortheasymmetricaction,theexemplarwasheldinonehandbytheendwiththesmallerbumpandmovedinalinearmotionforwardandbackward.Forthesymmetricaction,theex-emplarwasheldbytwohands,oneoneachbumpandrotatedbackandforth.Intheno-actionstationarycondition,theexperimenterintroducedtheexemplarbyholdingitflatonthepalmL. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005) Fig. 4.The exemplar and the 6 test objects used in Experiment 2. ofonehandandthensettingitonthetable.Allotheraspectsoftheprocedureanddesignwere3.2.Results and discussionFig.5showstheresultsfromtheforced-choiceprocedure;again,performinganactionbutnotwatchinganactionbeingperformedalteredchildren’sjudgmentsofshape.Intheactionconditions,childrenfavoredthesymmetricobjectwhentheyhadmovedtheexemplarsymmetricallyaboutitscenterbutnotwhentheyhadactedontheexemplarinawaythattreatedthetwosidesdifferently,(1,22)=38.30,.001.Thepreferencefortheshapecongruentwiththeactionalsoincreasedwiththemagnitudeofstimulusdifferencebetweenthetestobjectsandtheexemplar,(2,44)=3.48,.04.Therewerenodifferencesbetweenthethreeno-actionconditions;inallthreeconditions,therewasapreferenceforthemoresymmetrictestobjectineachpairandthispreferenceincreasedwiththegreatersymmetryandasymmetryofthetestobjectstotheexemplar,(2,44)=5.45,.01.Thisresultmayindicateageneralbaselinepreferenceforsymmetricaloverasymmetricalforms(seealsoRock,1973).Fig.6showstheresultsfromtheyes–noprocedure;againthefindingisthatactionalterschildren’scategorizationsofshape.Intheactionconditions,childrensaid“yes”thenameap-pliesmoretothesymmetrictestobjectswhentheyhadmovedtheexemplarsymmetricallywithtwohands,butsaid“yes”thenameappliesmoretotheasymmetrictestobjectswhentheyhadheldonepartoftheexemplarandmoveditwithonehand,(1,22)=25.93,.001.Childrenalsosaid“yes”tothetestobjectslessdifferentinoverallshapetotheexemplar,44)=13.70,.001.Theno-actionconditionsdidnotdifferfromoneanother;inallthreecon-ditions,childrensaid“yes”mosttoobjectsthatdifferedlessfromtheexemplarinoverallshapethantothosethatdifferedmore,(2,66)=19.78,.001,andtosymmetricmorethanasym-Again,underbothtaskprocedures,actionchangestherangeofshapeschildrenjudgetobemembersofthesamecategory,butwatchingsomeoneelseactonandmovetheobjectsinthesame way does not.4.General DiscussionTheoriesofobjectrecognition(bothstructuralandview-based)areforthemostparttheoriesofstaticobjectrecognition(seeLui&Cooper,2003).Yethowweactonobjectsisintimatelyrelatedtotheirshapes,andperhapsevendefiningofthem.Attheveryleast,theseresultsshowthatactionhasastronginfluenceontherangeofshapes2-year-oldstakeasbeingsimilarandappearstodosobydefiningaxesofelongationandsymmetry.Becauseelongationandsymmetryarecrucialtoshapeprocessingmoregenerally,thisraisesthepossibilitythatactionmoldsprocessesfundamentaltoobjectrecognition.Thecriticalopenquestionisthemechanism that underlies the observed effects. Four possibilities are outlined.L. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005) L. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005) Fig.6.Meanproportion“yes”responsesasafunctionofthesimilarityofthetestobjecttotheexemplarinthetwo directionaswell.Emergingevidencesuggestsexuberantmultimodalconnectionsinyoungbrainsthatareprunedwithdevelopment(Huttenlocher,2002;Johnson&Vecera,1996;Maurer&Mondloch,inpress;Thelen&Smith,1994).Ifthisisso,actionmightwellactivatevisualbrain regions in young children and play a role in the development of those regions.Clearlymoreexperimentsareneededtodisentangleandelucidatepossiblemechanisms.Theimportanceoftheseresults—andthecandidatemechanisms—concernstheirpotentialdevelopmentalconsequencesinthecontextofchildren’severydayandrepeatedinteractionswithobjects.Youngchildrenhabituallyactoncategoriesofobjectsinsystematicways.Theserepeatedactionsmaybeexpectedtochangehowaspectsofshapeareprocessedandremembered.Theimplicationofthisisclear:Ifwedidhabituallyusechairstotamelionsratherthanto sit on, we would represent their shapes differently than we do now.AcknowledgmentsThisresearchwassupportedbyR01HD28675–10fromtheNationalInstitutesofHealth.IthankLorindaCagle,ReginaHildebrand,andEmilyUnikelforthecreationofthestimuliandReferencesBiederman,I.(1987).Recognition-by-components:Atheoryofhumanimageunderstanding.PsychologicalRe-view, 94,Bucher,N.M.,&Palmer,S.E.(1985).Effectsofmotiononperceivedpointingofambiguoustriangles.Perception& Psychophysics, 38,Creem,S.H.,&Proffitt,D.R.(2001).Graspingobjectsbytheirhandles:AnecessaryinteractionbetweencognitionJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27,Duvdevani-Bar,S.,&Edelman,S.(1999).VisualrecognitionandcategorizationonthebasisofsimilaritiestomulInternational Journal of Computer Vision,33,Edelman,S.,&Intrator,N.(2003).Betterlimitedsystematicityinhandthanstructuraldescriptionsinthebush:ACognitive Science, 27Ellis,R.,&Tucker,M.(2000).Micro-affordance:Thepotentiationofcomponentsofactionbyseenobjects.Journal of Psychology, 91,Faillenot,I.,Toni,I.,Decety,J.,Gregoire,M.,&Jeannerod,M.(1997).Visualpathwaysforobject-orientedactionand object recognition: Functional anatomy with PET.Cerebral Cortex, 7,Gelman,S.A.,&Bloom,P.(2000).YoungchildrenaresensitivetohowanobjectwascreatedwhendecidingwhatCognition, 76,Principles of perceptual learning and development.New York: Academic.The senses considered as perceptual systems.Oxford, England: Houghton Mifflin.Glenberg,A.M.,Robertson,D.A.,Kaschak,M.P.,&Malter,A.J.(2003).EmbodiedmeaningandnegativeprimBehavioral Brain Sciences, 26,Grezes,J.,&Decety,J.(2001).Functionalanatomyofexecution,mentalsimulation,observation,andverbgeneraHuman Brain Mapping, 12,Hummel,J.E.(2001).Complementarysolutionstothebindingprobleminvision:Implicationsforshapeperception and object recognition.Neural binding of space and time.Visual Cognition, 8,Huttenlocher,P.R.(2002).Neuralplasticity:Theeffectsofenvironmentonthedevelopmentofthecerebralcortex.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.L. B. Smith/Cognitive Science29(2005)