/
PavlovianConditioningHowexcitationandinhibitionDetermineIdeomotionAdel PavlovianConditioningHowexcitationandinhibitionDetermineIdeomotionAdel

PavlovianConditioningHowexcitationandinhibitionDetermineIdeomotionAdel - PDF document

eloise
eloise . @eloise
Follow
342 views
Uploaded On 2022-08-24

PavlovianConditioningHowexcitationandinhibitionDetermineIdeomotionAdel - PPT Presentation

AthesispresentedforthedegreeofDoctorofPhilosophyCardi11UniversitySchoolofPsychologyNovember2019 SummaryAssociativetheoriesassumeasimpleordinalmappingbetweenthestrengthofanassociationbetweenacondit ID: 941126

cation 001 tracking classi 001 cation classi tracking cationandblock blockandlever wellentries 2016 trackers mackintosh pearce rescorla wagner cationandlever trackingandgoal

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Pdf The PPT/PDF document "PavlovianConditioningHowexcitationandinh..." is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

PavlovianConditioningHowexcitationandinhibitionDetermineIdeomotionAdelaFlorentinaIliescu AthesispresentedforthedegreeofDoctorofPhilosophyCardi UniversitySchoolofPsychologyNovember,2019 SummaryAssociativetheoriesassumeasimpleordinalmappingbetweenthestrengthofanassociationbetweenaconditionedstimulus(CS)andanunconditionedstimulus(US)andconditionedbehaviourinanexperimentalpreparation.Recentstudiesthathavetakenmultiplemeasuresofconditionedbehaviourchallengethisassumption.Thepur-poseofthisthesisisabetterunderstandingofthenatureoftheseindividualdi erencesinPavlovianconditioning,combiningempiricalevidencewiththeoreticaldevelopment.Ithasbeenobservedthatsimpleauto-shapingproceduresresultinmarkedindividualdi erences:someratsshowlearningbyinteractingwiththesign(sign-trackers,STs),othersbyinteractingwiththefood-wellorthegoal(goal-trackers,GTs).InChapter2,Iexaminedthesensitivityofthesetwobehaviours(sign-trackingandgoal-tracking)tochangingcontingencies.InbothSTsandGTs,US-orientedbehaviourwasmoresensitivetocontingencychangesthanCS-orientedbehaviour.Mostattemptstoex-plainthisdissociationhaveappealedtoadual-mechanismsapproach.InChapter3,Ipresentanewtheoreticalmodel,HeiDI,whichintegrateslearningandperformancefromasingle-processperspective.InChapter4,Iexaminetwoofthesepredictions.The rstpredictionrelatestohowtheUS-valuea ectsthedistributionofconditionedbehaviour.AccordingtoHeiDI,ahigherUS-valuewillresultinhigherlevelsofgoal-trackingincontrastwithlowerUSvalue.Experiment3suggestedthatthatahigherUS-valueresultsinmoreCS-orientedbehaviour,howeverthiswasnotreplicatedinExperiment4.Thesecondpredictionconcernsananalysisof

thefeaturepositivee ect,wherethediscriminationemergesmorereadilyforafeaturepositivedesign.HeiDIpredictsthatafeaturepositivee ectshouldbemoreevidentinCS-orientedbehaviour.Experiments4and5addressedthisprediction,howevertheanimalsdidnotshowlearninginthefeaturenegativedesign.Theimplicationsofthenew ndingsandthenewmodelarediscussedinChapter5.1 AcknowledgementsIwouldliketo rstexpressmygratitudetomysupervisors,ProfessorRobHoneyandProfessorDominicDwyer,fortheguidance,patience,encouragementandsupportduringthesewonderfulyears.IcannotexpresshowluckyIfeeltohaveyouasmymentors.Sergio,thankyouforsupportingmethroughthegoodandthebad.Youhavegivenmethepowertoovercomealltheobstacles.YouhavebeenmyinspirationandIwouldnothaveachievedthiswithoutyou.IwouldalsoliketothankallmycolleaguesfromtheBNL,Patricia,Sophie,andout-sidethe`bananaworld',Silvia,Oana,forbeingincrediblysupportiveandunderstanding.Thankyouforthe`shoulder'.Mami,multumescpentrusprijinulacordat^ntotiacestiani.Multumescpentruexemplulacordat,pentruc~ami-aifostmereual~aturi,pentrusfaturisiinspiratie.2 DeclarationThisworkhasnotbeensubmittedinsubstanceforanyotherdegreeorawardatthisoranyotheruniversityorplaceoflearning,norisbeingsubmittedconcurrentlyincandidatureforanydegreeorotheraward.Signed:AdelaF.IliescuDate18/09/2019STATEMENT1Thisthesisisbeingsubmittedinpartialful lmentoftherequirementsforthedegreeofPhD.Signed:AdelaF.IliescuDate18/09/2019STATEMENT2Thisthesisistheresultofmyownindependentwork/investigation,exceptwhereother-wisestated.Othersourcesareacknowledgedbyexplicitreferences.Theviewsexpressedaremyown.Signed:AdelaF.Ilies

cuDate18/09/2019STATEMENT3Iherebygiveconsentformythesis,ifaccepted,tobeavailableforphotocopyingandforinter-libraryloan,andforthetitleandsummarytobemadeavailabletooutsideorganisations.Signed:AdelaF.IliescuDate18/09/2019STATEMENT4:PREVIOUSLYAPPROVEDBARONACCESSIherebygiveconsentformythesis,ifaccepted,tobeavailableforphotocopyingandforinter-libraryloansafterexpiryofabaronaccesspreviouslyapprovedbytheAcademicStandards&QualityCommittee.Signed:AdelaF.Iliescu3 Date18/09/2019LargepartsofChapter2,3and5appearin:Iliescu,A.F.,Hall,J.,Wilkinson,L.S.,Dwyer,D.M.,&Honey,R.C.(2018).ThenatureofphenotypicvariationinPavlovianconditioning.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalLearningandCognition,44(4),358{369.Honey,R.C.,Dwyer,D.M.,&Iliescu,A.F.(2019).HeiDI:AmodelforPavlovianlearningandperformancewithreciprocalassociations.PsychologicalReview(inrevi-sion).IcontributedtotheconceptualdevelopmentofHeiDI,andtoitsformalpresentationforpublication.IalsobuilttheHeiDIapp,whichgeneratesthesimulatedresults,andderivedpredictionsfromit.TheaccuracyofthesepredictionswastestedinChapter4.4 Contents1GeneralIntroduction101.1TheoreticalanalysesofPavlovianlearningandbehaviour.........131.2Individualdi erencesinconditionedbehaviour...............141.3Conditionsthata ectsignandgoaltracking................161.4Theoreticalbackground............................191.4.1S-SvsS-Rlearning..........................201.4.2Behavioursystemstheory......................221.4.3Reinforcementlearning........................241.5ThesisOutline.................................292Investigatingthenatureofphenotypicvariation312.1Introduction...............................

...312.2Experiment1.................................352.2.1Method................................352.2.2Results.................................392.2.3DiscussionExperiment1.......................452.3Experiment2.................................452.3.1Method................................462.3.2Results.................................492.3.3DiscussionExperiment2.......................552.4Chapter2Discussion.............................553HeiDI:AmodelforPavlovianlearningandperformancewithrecipro-calassociations593.1TheRescorla-WagnerModel.........................603.2Individualdi erences.............................623.3HeiDI:Rationale,architectureandoverarchingassumptions........633.4Learningrules.................................675 3.4.1Excitatorylearninganderrorcorrection..............683.4.2Extinction...............................723.4.3Inhibitorylearning..........................743.5Performancerules...............................743.5.1Individualdi erencesin US.....................763.6Simulationsoflearningandperformance..................793.6.1Excitatoryconditioningandextinction...............793.6.2ChangeinUSvalue..........................823.6.3Compoundconditioningandthepoolederrorterm........833.6.4Combiningstimuliwithdi erentassociativehistories.......853.6.5Blocking:Learningandperformance................913.6.6Latentinhibition:Analternativeassociativeanalysis.......963.7Listofequations...............................994TestingpredictionsofHeiDI1004.1Individualdi erencesin US.........................1004.1.1Introduction..............................1004.1.2Experiment3............

.................1014.1.3Experiment4.............................1094.1.4DiscussionExperiments3and4...................1164.2Featurepositivee ect............................1174.2.1Introduction..............................1174.2.2Experiment5:Levercondition....................1204.2.3Experiment6:Lightcondition....................1324.2.4DiscussionExperiments4and5...................1375GeneralDiscussion1395.1Empirical ndings...............................1405.2Theoreticalimplications:HeiDI.......................1446 5.2.1Conditions,contentandperformance................1455.2.2Associationsbetweenthecomponentsofacompound.......1465.2.3Elementalandcon guralprocesses.................1485.2.4Limitationsandfurtherdevelopment................1495.2.5Concludingcomments........................1506Appendix1526.1ReproducibilityandOpenScienceFramework(OSF)...........1526.2HeiDIapp...................................1526.3Additionalnotes...............................1547 ListofFiguresFigure1:Distributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourforExper-iment1....................................39Figure2:ResultsfromExperiment1:thee ectsofareversalprocedureonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.......................40Figure3:CorrelationresultsfromExperiment1:thee ectsofareversalprocedureonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking................41Figure4:Distributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourforExper-iment2....................................49Figure5:ResultsfromExperiment2:thee ectsofaswitchinUSvalueonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.......................50Figure6:CorrelationresultsfromExperiment2:thee ectsof

aswitchinUSvalueonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking..................51Figure7:Aschematicforassociativestructuresthatunderpinthetranslationofexcitatorylearningintoperformance...................64Figure8:HeiDI:Simulationsofthee ectsofanextinctionprocedureonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking..........................81Figure9:SimulationsofHeiDIforthee ectsofachangeinUSvalueonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.......................83Figure10:SimulationsofHeiDIofthechangesinassociativestrengthwhencombiningstimuliwithdi erentassociativehistories............86Figure11:SimulationsofHeiDIofthechangesinassociativestrengthduringstandardblockinganddownshiftunblockingprocedures..........94Figure12:Distributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourforEx-periment3...................................103Figure13:SimulationofHeiDIforindividualdi erencesin USonsignandgoal-tracking.................................1048 Figure14:ResultsfromExperiment3:thee ectofdi erentvaluesof USonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.......................105Figure15:ResultsfromExperiment4:thee ectofdi erentvaluesof USonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking(replicationofExperiment3).......111Figure16:Distributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourforEx-periment4...................................112Figure17:SimulationsofHeiDIofafeaturepositivee ectwhenparametersarearrangedforabiastowardsRCS.....................122Figure18:SimulationsofHeiDIofafeaturepositivee ectwhenparametersarearrangedforabiastowardsRUS.....................123Figure19:Distributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourforEx-periment5.................

..................124Figure20:ResultsfromExperiment5:thee ectsofafeaturepositivedesignonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking......................125Figure21:ResultsfromExperiment5splitbytypeofstimuli:AvsBvsB(AB)126Figure22:Distributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourforEx-periment6...................................134Figure23:ResultsfromExperiment6:thee ectsofafeaturepositivedesignonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking......................1359 Chapter1GeneralIntroduction\Givemeadozenhealthyinfants,well-formed,andmyownspeci edworldtobringthemupinandI'llguaranteetotakeanyoneatrandomandtrainhimtobecomeanytypeofspecialistImightselect|doctor,lawyer,artist,merchant-chiefand,yes,evenbeggar-manandthief,regardlessofhistalents,penchants,tendencies,abilities,vocations,andraceofhisancestors"(Watson,1924,p.104).Watsonbelievedthatexperienceortrainingwastheprincipalfactorthatcontributestowhatapersonbecomes.ItisdiculttobelievethatanyonecouldnothaveacceptedatleastsomeversionofWatson'sclaim,thatdi erenttrainingallowspeopletobecomedoctors,lawyers,artists,merchant-chiefs.Accordingtothisview,allindividualdi er-encesinbehaviourre ectdi erentlearningexperiences.However,thefocusofthisthesisisonwhatisunacknowledgedbyWatson'sstatement,namelythatmaterialindividualdi erencesstillemerge,inspiteofsharedexperienceandtraining(e.g.,Flagel,Akil,&Robinson,2009;Patitucci,Nelson,Dwyer,&Honey,2016).Akeyquestionthatthisthesiswilladdress,iswhethertheseindividualdi erencesre ectdi erencesinthecontentoflearning(whatislearntinagivensituation)asop-posedtopossibledi erencesinthemechanismsoflearningbetweensubjects.In

ordertoaddressthiskeyquestion,oneneedstounderstandwhatislearnt(thecontentoflearning)inagivensituation,whatisthemechanismunderlyingthatlearning,andhowwhathasbeenlearntistranslatedintobehaviour(Rescorla,1988).Beforeaddressingtheseissues,Iwill rstbrie yreviewtheconceptoflearningfromanassociativeperspec-tive,beforeturningtoindividualdi erencesinaubiquitousformoflearning(Pavlovianconditioning)andoutlinecontemporaryaccountsofsuchdi erences.Theformalstudyofanimallearninghasalonghistoryanditispossibletoiden-tifytwomainapproachesthathaveledtowhatwenowknow.The rstapproachwasconcernedwiththestudyofadaptivebehaviourfromanevolutionarypointofview,10 contrastingadaptivespecialisationofabilitiestotheenvironmentofdi erentspecies(Mackintosh,1974,p.2).Thiscomparativeapproachfocussedonnaturallyoccurringacquiredbehaviours(e.g.,thedevelopmentofbirdsongand lialimprinting),andcanbecontrastedwiththestudyoftheprocessesunderlyingmodi cationsofbehaviourinprocedureschosentoinvolvemorearbitrarystimuli,responsesandtherelationshipsbetweenthem.Thelatterapproachisexempli edbytheexperimentalinvestigationofchangesinbehaviourandtheconditionswhenthesechangesoccurinlaboratorytasks,whichhaveattemptedtorevealgeneralprinciplesoflearning.Thisformofinvestigationinvolvedattemptstounderstandtheprocessesinvolvedinlearning,orthegeneralprin-ciplesinoperation,throughananalysisofwhensuchchangesinbehaviouroccur.Inthiscase,changesinbehaviourcanbeseenasboththethingtobeunderstoodandthemeansofdoingso,whichhasattendantproblems.Forexample,ifoneassumesthatthechangeinbehaviourbroughtaboutbyaPavlovianconditioningprocedurere ectsanassociation

betweentheeventsthathavebeenpresented(e.g.,achimingbellandfood),thenoneneedstospecifytheperformancerule(s)bywhichthatassociationisrevealedinbehaviour.Infact,asIwillshow,theoriesofassociativelearninghavetypicallyo eredonlythemostimpoverishedofperformancerules.Inthe eldofanimallearning,Mackintosh(1974)notedtwohistorical gures,PavlovandThorndike,whoprofoundlyshapeddecadesofresearch,basedontheirinitialproce-duresandtheoreticalinsights.Fromtheiroriginalexperimentalwork,twoexperimentalparadigmshavedeterminedthecourseofmostsubsequentresearchonlearning:clas-sicalconditioning(Pavlov)andinstrumentalconditioning(Thorndike).Notethatthedistinctionbetweenclassicalandinstrumentalconditioningreferstotheexperimentalarrangementstostudylearning(operationallytherulesthatdeterminedthedeliveryofareinforcerorreward),butdoesnotnecessarilyimplydi erentlearningprocesses.Experimentsonclassicalconditioningarethoseinwhichthecontingenciesarearrangedbetweenstimulipresentedindependentlyoftheactionsofananimal(e.g.,astimulusprecedinganoutcome)andthoseoninstrumentalconditioning,thecontingenciesare11 arrangedbetweentheactionsofananimalandotherevents(e.g.,aresponsehasthescheduledconsequenceofanoutcome).Thorndike'slawofe ectstatedthatanybehavioursthatarefollowedbypositiveconsequencesarelikelytoberepeatedandtheonesfollowedbynegativeconsequencesarelikelytobesuppressed(Thorndike,1927).Ininstrumentalconditioning,learningisgenerallyassessedbymeasuringthebehaviourorsetofbehavioursthatarefollowedbyreinforcement,andanincrease(ordecrease)inrespondingistakenasevidenceoflearning.Inclassicalconditioningthesubjectispresentedwitharelationshipbe

tweenastimulusandanoutcome(e.g.,atonefollowedbyfood)andchangesinbehaviouroccurswithoutanyobviouscontingenciesbetweenthebehaviouritselfandtheoutcomethatfollowsit.InatypicalclassicalorPavlovianconditioningprocedure,aneutralcondi-tionedstimulus(CS)ispairedwithamotivationallysigni cantunconditionedstimulus(USe.g.,food),whichproducesunconditionedresponses(UR;salivation).AsaresultofpairingtheCSandUStheCScomestoevokeaconditionedresponse(CR).Thisfundamentallearningprocesswas rstformallydocumentedbyPavlov(1927),whono-ticedthatdogswouldsalivateinthepresenceofcuesthatpredictedbeingfed(e.g.,theexperimentercomingintotheroom).ToexplainwhyaCScomestoevokeaCR,Pavlov(1930)suggestedthattheanimaltreatstheCSasitwouldbetheUS(stimulussubstitutiontheory).Thisanalysispre-dictsthattheCSwillcometoevokearesponsegreatlyresemblingthatevokedbytheUS,asitappearedtodoinPavlov'soriginalstudiesandmanyofthosethatfollowed.Forexample,inpigeonautoshaping,whentheCSisanilluminatedkeyandtheUSisthedeliveryofgraintoafood-well,theCRtothekeylightresemblestheURtothedeliveryoffood(i.e.,pecking).Moreover,anexperimentbyJenkinsandMoore(1973)manipulatedthenatureoftheUS(grainorwater)inapigeonautoshapingprocedureandshowedthatwhentheUSwasgrainthepresentationofthekeylightelicited\foodpecks"whereaswhenitwaswaterthekeylightelicited\waterpecks":theCRsresem-bledthedistinctconsummatoryresponsestofoodandwater.However,theprinciple12 ofstimulussubstitutionischallengedbyconditioningprocedureswhichresultinaCRthatisdi erentfromtheUR.Forexample,whenratsaregivenpairingsofaCSwithelectricshock(theUS),whiletheCRisoftenacessationofallactivity(i.e.,freezing),theUSitselfiniti

allyprovokesaburstinactivity(e.g.,Blanchard,Fukunaga,&Blan-chard,1976).Whiletheideaofstimulussubstitutionhasbeenchallenged,contemporarytheoriesofPavlovianconditioning,thefocusofthisthesis,havedonelittlebywayofaddressinghow(associative)learningbecomesevidentinacquiredbehaviour.1.1TheoreticalanalysesofPavlovianlearningandbehaviourMorerecenttheoreticaltreatmentsofPavlovianconditioninghavefollowedPavlov'sleadinassumingthatpairingsofaCSwithaUSresultintheformationofanassociationbetweenthecentralrepresentationsoftheevents(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner,1981).Thisproposition{thatthecriticalassociationisstimulus-stimulus(S-S)ratherthanbetweenastimulusandresponse(S-R){carrieswithitaneedtospecifyhowlearningistranslatedintoperformance.TheRescorla-Wagnermodelremainsoneofthemostin uentialinthe eld,anditsapplicationtotheissuesthatarecentraltothisthesiswillbeoutlinednext,includingitsapproachtoobservedperformance.TheRescorla-WagnerModel(Rescorla&Wag-ner,1972)assumesthatconditionedbehaviourre ectstheformationofanassociationbetweentherepresentationsactivatedbytheCSandUS.ThepresentationoftheCScomestoassociativelyactivatetherepresentationoftheUSandtherebybehaviour.Thechangeintheassociativestrength(VCS�US)ofaCSonagiventrialisdeterminedbythedi erencebetweenthemaximumassociativestrengthsupportablebyaUS()andthepooledassociativestrengthofallstimulipresentedonthattrial(PVTotal�US).ThecurrentassociativestrengthofthatstimulusVCS�USisthesumofVCS�USontheprevioustrialandthechangeinassociativestrengthonthecurrenttrial(VCS�US).13 Therateoflearningismodulatedbytwolearningparame

ters CSand USandthemodelassumesthat CSand USarerelatedtothephysicalproperties(i.e.,salienceorintensity)oftheCSandUS.IwilldiscusstheRescorla-ModelinmoredetailinChapter3.However,theresultingequationforthechangeinlearningonagiventrialproposedbythismodeltakestheform:VCS�US= CS US(-PVTotal�US).AconsequenceofadoptingtheviewthatPavlovianconditioninginvolvesS-Sassoci-ations(asopposedtoS-Rassociations)isthatmodelsliketheonedevelopedbyRescorlaandWagner(1972)neededtoprovidesomeanalysisofhowsuchassociationsaretrans-latedintoobservablebehaviour.Thisneedissharedbyotherformalassociativelearningmodels(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Wagner,1981).Ofcourse,asso-ciativetheoristsarenotblindtothisissue,andsohavetypicallyo eredthesimplifyingassumptionthatrespondingelicitedbytheassociativeactivationofarepresentationofaUSismonotonicallyrelatedtothestrengthoftheunderlyingassociation.Whilethisassumptionhasservedforsometime,Iwillnextdiscusstheresultsofrecentstudies,whichposeachallengetoanylearningtheorythatassumesamonotonicrelationshipbetweenlearningandperformance.1.2Individualdi erencesinconditionedbehaviourIndividualdi erenceswereacentralfocusofpsychologistsintheearly20thcentury,butthestudyofindividualdi erencesinanimallearninghasplayedlittleroleinthedevelopmentofcontemporarytheory(Matzel&Sauce,2017).Perhapsthisfactre ectedtheviewthatthecontrolledandsharedenvironmentsandrestrictedgeneticbackgroundofmanylaboratoryanimalsshouldlimittheextentofindividualdi erencesinlearntbehaviourbetweenthem.However,recentevidencesuggeststhatPavlovianconditioningprocedurescanproducemarkedindiv

idualdi erencesinconditionedbehaviour.Forexample,Patitucci,Nelson,Dwyer,andHoney(2016,Experiment2)presentedratswithtwolevers.Onelever(e.g.,leftlever),thereinforcedlever,wasinsertedintheexperimentalchamberfor10sandwasimmediatelyfollowedbyaccesstosucroseas14 reward.Thecontrollever(e.g.,rightlever)ontheotherhand,wasinsertedinthechamber,howeveritwasnotreinforced.Theratsreceivedtwentytrialsofeachleverfor12days.Someratsshowedthattheyhadlearnttherelationshipbetweenthereinforcedleverandfoodbypredominantlyinteractingwiththelever(orthesign)andothersbyinvestigatingthefood-well(orthegoal),wherethereinforcerwasabouttobedelivered.Likeothers,Irefertothebehaviourofengagingwiththelever(typicallyautomaticallyrecordedasleverpresses)assign-tracking,andtotheanimalsthatshowatendencytowardsthesignortheleverassign-trackers(STs).Similarly,Irefertothebehaviourofengagingwiththefood-well(typicallyautomaticallyrecordedasfood-wellentries)asgoal-tracking,andtotheanimalsthatshowatendencytoengagewiththegoalasgoal-trackers(GTs)(Brown&Jenkins,1968;Hearst&Jenkins,1974).InthestudyconductedbyPatituccietal.(2016,Experiment2),inwhichtheinser-tionofoneleverwasfollowedbysucroseandtheinsertionofanother(controllever)wasnot,amediansplitwasusedtoseparateratsintotwogroups(STsandGTs)onthebasisofwhethertheiractivityduringthe nalblockoftrainingwaspredominantlydirectedto-wardstheleverorfood-well.Thisanalysisallowedthedevelopmentofthesign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourstobetracedacrosstraining.However,analysisatthelevelofindividualratsrevealsthatthebiastowardssign-trackingorgoal-trackingwasrelativelycontinuousinnature.Whenleveractivityorsign-trackingwas

usedtoasseslearning,theSTsshowedbetterlearningthantheGTs;butwhenfood-wellactivityorgoal-trackingisusedthenthereverseistrue.Focussingononemeasure(e.g.,sign-tracking)leadstotheconclusionthatlearninghadproceededmorereadilyinonesetofratsthantheother,whilefocussingonthesecondmeasure(e.g.,goal-tracking)leadstotheoppositeconclusion.Asitwillbediscussedinlengthinfuturesections,learningtheoriesthatassumeamonotonicrelationshipbetweenlearningandacquiredbehaviour(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner,1981)areunabletoexplainthisdissociationintheexpressionoflearning:thereisnosimplymonotonicmappingbetweenthestrengthoftheCS-USassociationandthedi erent15 levelsofleverpressandfood-wellentriesseenacrosssign-andgoal-trackinganimals.Theseexperimentshighlighttheimportanceofhowlearningisassessed,howittrans-latesinperformance,andthefactthatonemeasurementalonemightnotbeenoughtodrawgeneralconclusionsaboutwhattheanimalhaslearntinanygivensetofcircum-stances.Inthefollowingsection,IdiscussstudiesonPavlovianconditioningwheremultiplemeasurementsofconditionedbehaviourhavebeencontrasted,whichsuggeststhatsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehavioursarea ecteddi erentlybydi erenttypesofmanipulations.1.3Conditionsthata ectsignandgoaltrackingThisthesisisabouthowlearningistranslatedintobehaviour.Therehasbeenagreatdealofresearchconcerningsign-andgoal-tracking,althoughnotnecessarilyinthecontextofindividualdi erences.Iwillfocusnextontheexperimentalmanipulationsthata ectsign-andgoaltracking.Considerationofthee ectsofthesemanipulationsarekeytothedevelopmentofanygeneraltheorythatintegrateslearningandperfo

rmance;suchtheorywillbepresentedinChapter3.StudiesbyWasserman,Franklin,andHearst(1974)showthatpositivecontingencybetweentheCSandUSisneededforthetwocriticalbehaviourstoemerge.Wassermanetal.(1974)exposedpigeonstoeitherapositivecontingency(alightedkeyCSwhichwasalwaysfollowedbygraindelivery,US),negativecontingency(theCSandUSwereexplicitlyunpaired),backwardspairing(theUSwaspresentedbeforetheCS)orCSonly(theCSwasneverfollowedbyanygraindelivery)procedure.Theonlybirdsthatshowedsign-tracking,bypeckingatthelightedkey,weretheonesexposedtoapositivecontingencybetweentheCSandUS.Onthebasisofkey-peckingmeasurementalonethebehaviourofbirdsintheothergroupscouldnotbedi erentiatedfromeachother.However,onthebasisofapproach-withdrawalmeasurements,thegroupsofbirdscouldbeclearlydistinguished.BirdsintheCSonlyandbackwardpairingsgroupdidnotdisplayanyapproach-withdrawalbehaviour,whilebirdsinthenegativecontingency16 groupspenttheirtimeontheothersideofthechamber,awayfromtheCS.EvenifWassermanetal.(1974)didnotmeasuregoal-trackingdirectly,theirmeasurementsofapproachandwithdrawalseemtosuggestthatthesameistrueforgoal-tracking,aspigeonsspenttheirtimeawayfromthesiteofUSdelivery,makingithardforthemtoshowanygoal-trackingbehavioureither.IntheCSpositivecontingencygroup,grainalwaysfollowedtheCS,howeverithasbeenseenthatpartialreinforcementofaslittleasof25percentoftheCStrials,producessign-trackingbehaviour(Wasserman,1974).Partialreinforcementincreasedthenumberoftrialstothe rstpeck,howeveritproducedhigherasymptoticsign-trackinglevels(Davey&Cleland,1982;Gottlieb,2004,2006).Incontrast,goal-trackingwassuppressedwithpartialreinforcementscheduleswhencompar

edwithacontinuouslyreinforcedgroup(Davey&Cleland,1982).PriorexperiencewiththeCSortheUSa ectsdi erentlysign-andgoal-trackingbehaviour.Interestingly,studiesinpigeonsshowedthatpreexposuretoCSalonetrialsbeforepairingwithaUSinterferedwiththedevelopmentofsign-tracking,butnotwithgoal-trackingresponsesdirectedatthesiteoffooddelivery(Boughner&Papini,2003).PreexposuretotheUSbeforepairingitwithaCSdelayedthedevelopmentofsign-tracking(Engberg,Hansen,Welker,&Thomas,1972),buthadnoe ectongoal-trackingbehaviour(Costa&Boakes,2009).InSection1.4,theoreticalbackground,Iwilldiscusstheproposedmechanismsunderlyingsign-andgoal-tracking,whichprovidesomeinsightintowhysomemanipulationsa ectonebehaviourmorethantheother.Morrison,Bamkole,andNicola(2015)changedthevalueoftheUSaftertheini-tialtraining(leverCS{sucroseUSpairings)bysubsequentlypairingtheUSwithillnessintheabsenceoftheCS(i.e.,conditionedtasteaversion).Thisprocedurere-sultedinenhanced,ratherthandiminished,sign-trackingbehaviour,butincontrast,areductioningoal-trackingbehaviour.Thisdissociationhascommonlybeenusedasevidencefordi erentialmechanismsunderlyingthesebehaviours,onerelyingonastimulus-stimulusassociation(goal-tracking)andtheotheronastimulus-responseasso-ciation(sign-tracking).IntheTheoreticalBackground,section1.4,Iwillreturntothis17 issueatmorelength.TemporalparametersbetweentheCSonsetando setandUSdeliveryhaveimpor-tante ectsonthestrengthofCRs.Ingeneral,theCRisstrongerthemorewidelyspacedthetrialsareandshorterthegapbetweenCSo setandUSdelivery(Hearst&Jenkins,1974).Withlongergapsbetweentrials(intertrialinterval,ITI)sign-trackingbehaviourisenhanced(e.g

.,Terrace,Gibbon,Farrell,&Baldock,1975)andwithshorterITIs,goal-trackingisenhanced(e.g.,Cinotti,Marchand,Roesch,Girard,&Khamassi,2019;Leeetal.,2018).IthasbeensuggestedthatvariationsinITIcouldproducethisdi erencethrougha ectingthe\predictiveness"or\informativeness"oftheCS(Hearst&Jenkins,1974).ShortITIscouldleadtotheCSnotbeingveryinformativeofwhenthereinforcerwillbedelivered.Incontrast,withlargeITIstheoccurrenceoftheCSisinformativefortheUSdelivery.CSdurationseemsnottoa ectthegeneraltopographyoftheresponse,durationsof3and8secondswereequallye ective(Brown&Jenkins,1968).ThephysicalpropertiesoftheCS,aswellasthenatureoftheUSa ecttheto-pographyoftheCRsevokedbytheCS.Furthermore,di erentspeciesdisplaydi erentpatternofCRs(Shettleworth,1972).AlocalisedCSgenerallyevokesmoresign-trackingthatadi useone,andtheresponsedependsonthetypeofCS,andhowitsupportssign-trackingbehaviour(alevercanbetouched,bitten,licked,howeveralightcanonlybepeckedororientedtowards,Leslie&Millenson,1996).ThelocationoftheCSinrelationwiththeUSdeliverysiteisalsoimportant.Generally,closespatialproximitybetweentheCSandtheUSdeliverysitesustainsmoresign-trackingbehaviour(Leslie&Millenson,1996).CRsevokedbyCSsdependalsoonthenatureoftheUS.JenkinsandMoore(1973)comparedfoodandwaterreinforcers.Mostpecksatwaterpredictivesignalswerechar-acterizedbyaveryslightopeningofthebeak,sometimeslickingandswallowingmove-ments,whichwereirregularlyspaced,sustainedandrelativelyweak.Incontrast,pecksatthefood-predictivesignalwerewiththebeakwide-open,evenlyspaced,strongand18 brief.RackhamHearstandJenkins(1974)observedthatiftheCSpredictstheoppor-tunitytomatein

malepigeons,sign-trackingbehaviourwascharacterisedbyapproach,nodding,cooing,circlingandbowing,typicalcourtshipbehaviour.Thereisstillmuchtolearnabouttheconditionsthatpromoteorreducesign-andgoal-trackingbehaviour.MostoftheexperimentsdescribedaboveassumethatanymeasurementofanyCRwouldleadtothesameconclusionaboutwhathasbeenlearnt(orthecurrentvalueofassociativestrength)inanygivenexperimentalprocedure.Thedi erentpatternsobservedwhenmultiplemeasurementsofdi erentCRshavebeentakenchallengethisassumption(e.g.,Patituccietal.,2016).InthefollowingsectionIfocusondi erentapproachesthathaveattemptedtoprovideanexplanationofthedi erentwaysinwhichlearningistranslatedintoconditionedbehaviour.1.4TheoreticalbackgroundGeneralprocessmodelsoflearning(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner,1981)focusonlearningandhowassociativestrengthchangesundervariousexperimentalconditionsandnotonhowlearningisexpressed.Accordingtosuchmodels,thepredictiverelationshipbetweentheCS(e.g.,lever)andUS(e.g.,food)shouldresultintheformationofanassociationbetweentherepresen-tationsofthetwoallowingfuturepresentationsofthelevertoactivateamemoryofthefood.Thisanalysisallowstheexactformofconditionedresponsetodi erfromthespeci cnatureoftheresponsetofooditself(Wagner&Brandon,1989),butdoesnotprovideacoherentaccountforindividualdi erencesinconditionedbehaviour.Thisisperhapsunsurprisinggiventhefactthatthesegeneral-processaccountswerenotin-tendedtoprovideacharacterizationofindividualdi erences.However,wehaveseenthatdi erentbehaviouralassessmentsoflearningleadtodi erentconclusionsaboutlearning(e.g.,Patituccieta

l.,2016).Mosttheoreticalapproacheshaveignoredtheissueofhowlearningistranslatedintoperformance.However,therearesomegeneralprinciplesthato eranexplanationforsomeofthechallengingresults.19 Oneoftheproposedmechanismstoexplainindividualdi erencesintheformPavlo-vianconditionedbehaviouriscentredonwhetheritisbasedonadirectassociationbetweentheCSandtheUS(stimulus-stimulusorS-Slearning)ortheCSisassociatedwiththeresponseelicitedbytheUS(stimulusresponseorS-Rlearning).Idiscusstheimplicationsofthisviewinthefollowingsection.1.4.1S-SvsS-RlearningFormaltheoriesofassociativelearningassumethatorganismsformassociationsbetweentherepresentationsofconditionedandunconditionedstimuli.Thesestim-ulus{stimulus(S-S)associationshavebeencontrastedwiththeformationofstimu-lus{response(S-R)associationsbetweentheprocessesactivatedbythestimulusandthemotorprogramforgeneratingaresponse(Hull,1943).Skinner(1948)suggestedthatsuperstitiousconditioningofresponsesdirectedto-wardstheCSmightexplainthedevelopmentandmaintenanceofsign-tracking.Onecouldarguethatitispossibleforpigeonsbeingexposedtoalightedkey,toperformasetofbehaviours(orienting,approaching,contacting)whichaccidentallyareassociatedwiththereinforcer,inanoperantconditioningfashion(S-R).Consistentwithview,Patituccietal.(2016)suggestedthatgoal-trackingcouldbebasedonanS-Slearningmechanismandsign-trackingonanS-Rmechanismsthatoperateinparallel,eachofthemcontrollingpartoftheCRrepertoire.Goal-trackingactivityseemstore ectthecurrentstatusoftherelationshipbetweentheleverandthereinforcer(i.e.,anS-Sassociation)asdevaluatingtheUSa ectsrats'tendencytoenterthefood-wellmorethanlever-pressingbehaviour(M

orrisonetal.,2015).Moreover,Patituccietal.(2016)observedthatthebiastowardapproachingthefood-wellrelativetoleverpressingwaspositivelycorrelatedwiththepalatabilityofthereinforcer,andthatsatingrodentswiththereinforcera ectsconditionedfood-wellactivitybutnotlever-orientedactivity.Evidencemotivatingtheviewofsign-trackingbehaviourre ectsaS-Rassociationcomesfromstudieswherecontingencychangesa ectlesssign-trackingthan20 goal-tracking(e.g.,extinction,devaluation).Ratskeeppressingtheleverregardlessofthecurrentstatusoftherelationshipbetweentheleverandthereinforcer,viewedashabitualbehaviour(e.g.,Ahrens,Singer,Fitzpatrick,Morrow,&Robinson,2016;Morrisonetal.,2015).However,thestudyconductedbyAhrensetal.(2016)needstobetreatedwithsomecautionastheydidnotmeasurebothlever-orientedandfood-wellorientedbehavioursinratsclassi edasSTsandGTs.Thusitisunclearwhetherthedissociationine ectsbetweenSTandGTanimalsistrulyadi erencebetweenindividualsthemselvesorisinsteadadi erenceinthespeci cbehavioursmeasuredineachsetofanimals.IwillcomebacktothisissueinmoredetailintheReinforcementLearningsectionandinChapter2,asthisistheaimoftheexperimentspresentedinthischapter.Theviewofsign-trackingasastimulus-responseassociationischallengedhoweverbyaninterestingexperimentconductedbyBrowne(1976),whichsuggeststhatsign-trackingemergesevenwhenresponsestowardstheCSarenotreinforced.Inthisex-periment,pigeonscouldonlyobservetherelationshipbetweentheCS(keylight)andUS(grain)butcouldnotphysicallyinteractwiththeCSoraccessthegrainsastheywereseparatedfromtheCSandUSbyatransparentPlexiglasscreen.Pigeonswereexposedtoeitherpositive,negativeornocorrelat

ionbetweenthepresentationsoftheCSandUS.Whensubsequentlygivenaccesstoboththekeylightandgrainhopper,theanimalsinthepositivecorrelationgroupinteractedwiththeCSfarmorethanthenocorrelationgroup.WhilethebirdsinthenegativecorrelationgroupbarelyinteractedwiththeCS.Visualinspectionofthebirds'behaviourduringtheobservationphasedidnotsuggestevidenceofsign-trackingresponses.ResponsestowardstheCSwereneverreinforcedduringthelearning(observation)phaseandyettheywererapidlyexpressedwhengiventheopportunity.Browne'sinterpretationofhispatternofresultswasthatitseemedimplausibletosupposetheanimalshavelearnttorespondtothekeylightthroughaprocessofstimulus-responselearning.EvidencefromomissionproceduresfromWilliamsandWilliams(1969),whereany21 peckatthelightedkeywouldterminatethetrialandpreventscheduledgrains,alsosuggeststhatresponse{reinforcerrelationsdonotexplainoratleastarenotsucienttofullyexplainsign-trackingbehaviourtowardstheCS.Pigeonsstillpeckedatthelightedkey,albeittoalesserdegree,evenifthisbehaviourwasneverreinforced.Theseexperimentssuggestthatsign-trackingemergesastheanimalslearnthere-lationshipbetweenthecuesandthereinforcer,andsecondly,sign-trackingisdirectedtowardsthemostpredictivecues.Sign-trackingemergesevenifcontactresponsesareneverpairedwithareinforcer,suggestingthatstimulus-responselearningisnotsolelyresponsibleforthisbehaviour,oratleastnotfortheacquisitionstage.However,thefactthatoutcomedevaluationorextinction(e.g.,Ahrensetal.,2016;Morrisonetal.,2015)a ectssign-trackinglessthangoal-trackingmightsuggestapossibleroleforstimulus-responseassociationinthemaintenanceofthebehaviour.Iwillpresentanalternativetheoreticalan

alysisfortheseandotherresultsinChapter3.Thistheoreticalframework(S-SvsS-R)addresseswhatitislearntduringPavlovianconditioning.However,itisnotabletoexplainwhysomeanimalsengagewiththesignandotherswithgoal.TheevidencesuggeststhatS-Slearningisnecessaryforacquisitionofsign-andgoal-tracking,buttheroleofS-Rlearningseemstobelessclearandcouldpotentiallycontributeatmaintainingsign-trackingbehaviour.However,itisuncleariftheseprocessesoperateinparallelor,ifthereisashift,whenitwouldoccur.Ifweassumethatgoal-trackingisbasedonS-Slearning,andsign-trackingonS-Rlearning(atleastduringthelaterstages),itwouldprovideageneralaccountforwhysign-trackingislesssensitivetochangethangoal-tracking,butstillleavesunexplainedtheoriginoftheseindividualdi erences:whysomeanimalsengageinS-SandsomeinS-Rlearning.TheexperimentsthatIpresentinChapter2aimtodevelopabetterunderstandingofthedi erentsensitivitytochangeofthesebehaviours,inSTsandGTs.1.4.2BehavioursystemstheoryTimberlake(1994)proposedthatananimalcomestoanylearningsituationwith22 anevolutionaryhistorythathasshapedpre-existinghierarchicallyorganizednetworkofbehaviour.Agivenmotivationalsystem(likethefeedingsystem)isformedofdif-ferentsub-systems(e.g.,predationorsocialforaging),modes(GeneralSearch,FocalSearch,andHandling/Consuming),andmorespeci cstimulus-response(S-R)modules.Afeedingsystemwouldbeanexhaustivesetofbehavioursavailabletotheanimalthatgenerallyhavethefunctionof ndingandconsumingfood.DuringPavloviancondi-tioning,theCRvariesaccordingofwhichmodegetsconditionedtotheCS.IftheCScomestoassociatewithageneralsearchmode,thenbehavioursthataredistanttothegoalobjectshouldbeconditioned.Howe

ver,iftheCSbecomesassociatedwithamorespeci cfocalsearchmode,thenbehavioursthataremoreproximaltothegoalobjectshoulddominate.Inoneexperiment,Timberlake,Wahl,andKing(1982)pairedaballbearingCSwithfoodwitha6s(long)CS-USintervalandtheynoticedthatratslearnedtoapproach,contact,andcarrytheballbearing.However,whentheyuseda2sCS-USinterval(short),theratsavoidedcontactwiththeballbearinganddirectlyapproachedthefoodmagazinewhentheballbearingCSappeared.Inotherwords,itlookedasthoughamoregeneralsearchmodewasconditionedwiththelongCS-USintervalandafocalsearchmodewasconditionedwithashortCS-USinterval.Thereisthepossibilitythatduringagivenconditioningproceduredi erentanimalsassociatetheCSwithdi erentmodes(e.g.,aSTassociatestheCSwithageneralsearchmodeandaGTwithamorefocalsearchmode)whichthenleadtotheobserveddi erencesinengagingwitheithertheCSorthegoallocation.Whythiswouldbethecaseremainsunclear.However,onepossibleexplanationcouldbetherelatedtodi erenceinanimals'perceptionofthetimingbetweentheCSandUS.Butwhilethebehavioursystemsapproachhasaclearvalueintermsofananalysisofperformance,itleaveslearninglargelyaside.Itdoesnotprovidetestablepredictionsinrelationtowhattheanimalhaslearntandhowitistranslatedintotheactivationofaspeci csystem.Furthermore,italsofailstopredictwhatmodeananimalwouldbeingivenanexperimentalprocedureorwhatleadstotheactivationofthatspeci csystem.23 1.4.3ReinforcementlearningComputationalanalysesoflearninghavepaidsubstantialattentiontothedistinc-tionbetweenmodel-freeandmodel-basedformsoflearning.Model-basedstrategiesareheldtogenerategoal-directedchoicesemployingamodelorcognitive-stylerepresenta-tion

,whichisaninternalmapofeventsandstimulifromtheexternalworld(Dickinson&Balleine,2002a).Theinternalmodelsupportsprospectiveassessmentoftheconse-quencesoftakingparticularactions.Bycontrast,model-freestrategieshavenomodelofoutsideevents,butinsteadmerelylearnbycachinginformationabouttheutilitiesofoutcomesencounteredonpastinteractionswiththeenvironment.LearninginModel-Freesystemsreliesonacomputedreinforcementsignal,therewardpredictionerror(Lesaint,Sigaud,Flagel,Robinson,&Khamassi,2014).Thisleadstodirectrulesforhowtobehave,orpropensitiesforperformingparticularactions(Dayan&Berridge,2014).Lesaintetal.(2014)modelledindividualdi erencesinPavlovianconditioningusingthisdual-processapproach.Theassumptionisthatratsusethetwolearningsys-tems,employingdistinctmechanismstolearnthesametask,withbothsystemspresentinanygivenanimalandthebalancebetweenthemisgivenbyaparameter(!).ForaST,thisbalancefavoursthemodel-freesystem,andforaGTitfavoursamodel-basedsystem.Thisapproachimpliesthatobserveddi erencesbetweensign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourareduetoanaturaltendencyoftheanimaltoengagewitheithertheCSorthelocationoftheUSorthegoal.STsarethoughttoattributetheCSwithincentivesalience,makingtheCSmoredesirable.Apotentialissuearisesfromthefactthatwhatdetermines!isunclear.Therewasnosuggestion!couldbepredictedbeforehand,resultinginasomethingofcircularargument:aSTisaSTbecauseitdisplayssign-trackingbehaviour(albeit,therearesuggestionsofotherindependently-assessabledi erencesbetweensign-andgoal-trackinganimals,suchastheirimpulsivityorsensitivitytoconditionedreinforcement,e.g.,Lovic,Saunders,Yager,&Robinson,2011).Dopamineisthoughttobecent

ralforreward-relatedprocesses,buttheexactna-24 tureofitsroleremainscontroversial(Schultz,2006).Phasicneurotransmissioninthemesolimbicdopaminesystemisinitiallytriggeredbythereceiptofreward(US),butshiftstoacuethatpredictsareward(CS)afterassociativelearning(whentherewardpredictionerrorisreduced;Flageletal.,2011;Schultz,Dayan,&Montague,1997).Lesaintetal.(2014)examinedphasicdopaminereleaseinthenucleusaccumbens,andobserveddopaminewasreleasedduringtheacquisitionofasign-trackingbutnotgoal-trackingresponse.ApotentialissuewiththisinterpretationarisesfromthefactthatdopaminereleaseismeasuredinSTswhiletheyarelever-pressingbutnotwhentheyareenteringthefood-well,andviceversafortheGTs.Fromthisstudyisunclearwhetherthedopaminereleaseisrelatedtothepropensityoftheanimaltoengagewiththeleverorthefood-well,orofthebehaviouritself(lever-pressingorfood-wellentries).Inanycase,thelinkbetweendopaminereleaseandsign-tracking,togetherwiththeapparently`non-adaptive'natureofthisbehaviour(e.g.,resistancetoextinctionorengagingwithaCSevenifthispreventsthedeliveryofarelevantfoodrewardforahungryanimal,Ahrensetal.,2016;Williams&Williams,1969)hasledtotheproposalthatSTsrepresentapotentialmodelforaddictionorimpulsivity(Bissonetteetal.,2015;Lovic,Saunders,Yager,&Robinson,2011;Ostlund&Balleine,2008).ThisviewassumesthatcuesassociatedwithrewardsnotonlyevokeCRsbuttheycanalsobecomeattractiveanddesirableintheirownright,actingasincentivestimuli.Alongthesamelines,Lesaintetal.(2014)assumethatdi erencesinsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourarebecauseofanaturaltendencyoftheanimaltoattributesomeCSswithincentivesalienceandmotivatebehaviour.Lovicetal.(2011)compared

STsontwotestsofso-calledimpulsiveaction(atwo-choiceserialreactiontimetaskandadi erentialreinforcementoflowratesofrespondingtask)andonetestofimpulsivechoice(adelaydiscountingchoiceprocedure).Forthe rsttest,theyinitiallytrainedratswithalever,thatwasalsoilluminated,followedbythedeliveryofonepellet.Subsequently,theydividedtheratsinSTsandGTsusing25 anindexwhichindicatedthepropensitytoapproacheithertheleverorthefood-well(seeMeyeretal.,2012).Forthesubsequentphasesoftheexperiment,theyonlyusedanimalsthatwouldpredominantlysign-orgoal-track.Duringthetwo-choiceserialre-actiontesttheypresentedratswithtwoilluminatednose-pokeports,ofwhichonlyonewasfollowedbyareinforcer.Responsesintotheilluminatednose-pokeportduringthetrialterminatedthetrialandresultedinthedeliveryofafoodpellet(forthecorrectport).Iftheanimalmadenoresponses,thenose-pokeportstayedilluminatedfor5sandwasfollowedbythereward.TheyfoundthatSTsmademoreprematureresponsesthanGTs.AproblemwiththisexperimentarisesfromtheshiftfromPavloviancon-ditioningtooperantconditioning.Inthe rststage,theleverwasreinforcedregardlessoftheanimal'sresponse,butinthesecond,anose-pokeintheport(aresponse)isnowreinforced.GTsalreadyhadapredominantbehaviourofengagingwiththefood-well,whiletheCSfortheSTshaschangedfromalevertonose-pokeport.Itisunclearhowthesechangesmighthavea ectedthegeneraltendencytointeractwiththefood-wellandtheresultsshouldbetobetreatedwithcaution.Forthesecondtestofimpulsiveaction,anothergroupofratswentthroughthesametrainingprocedure,afterwhichtheywereplacedonanFR1( xedratioof1response)operanttask,untiltheyreached100responses.Subsequently,theyweretestedonDRL-10s(d

i erentialreinforcementoflowratesofrespondingtask)schedulefor5daysandDRL-20sschedulefor15days.OntheDRL-10sandDRL-20sschedules,ratswerereinforcedonlyifatleast10sor20s,respectively,elapsedbetweenresponses.TheyfoundthatSTswerelessecientinthetaskthanGTs.Again,previoussign-trackingbehaviourintermsofleverinteractionmighthavea ectedtheresults,giventhetendencyinthetrainingphasetoengagewiththelever.Fortheirdelaydiscountingchoiceprocedure,theyfoundnodi erencesbetweensign-andgoal-trakinggroups.Onthebasisofthispatternofresults,theauthorsconcludethatSTsaremore`impulsive'thanGTs,therearehowever,alternativeinterpretations.Ahrens,Singer,Fitzpatrick,Morrow,andRobinson(2016)trainedratswithalever26 CSfollowedbyareinforcer.Thentheyintroducedperiodsofnon-reinforcement(ex-tinction)oftheCSandnoticedthatSTsextinguishedleverpressesmoreslowlythanGTsextinguishedfood-wellentries.TheyconcludedthatSTsareless exibleintheirbehaviour.ThisconclusionwasbasedonleverpressesdataforSTsandfood-wellentriesdatafortheGTs.Asnotedbefore,thefactthatSTandGTgroupswereonlyassessedintermsoftheirpredominantbehaviourmeansthatthedi erencein exibilitycouldbeattributedeithertothebehaviour(leverpressorfood-wellentry)ortheanimalsthemselves(STsorGTs).Thustofullysupporttheclaimthatthisre ectdi erencesintheanimals,itwouldbenecessaryforbothbehaviours(leverpressesandfood-wellentries)tobeassessedinanimalsclassi edasSTsandGTs.Itisunclearwhetherslowerextinctionisapropertyoftheanimalorthebehaviouritself,whichwillbediscussedatlengthinChapter2.Inanotherexperimentwhichinvestigatedthelinkbetweensign-trackingandaddic-tion,Pena-Oliveretal.(2015)examinedwh

etherselectivelybredalcohol-preferringandalcohol-non-preferringratsshowdi erentiallevelsofimpulsivityandconditionedbe-haviouralresponsestofoodincentives.Interestingly,theyfoundthatalcohol-preferringratsshowedmoregoal-orientedbehaviourthancontrolsandnoevidenceofimpulsivebehaviourwasfound.Partoftheargumentlinkingsign-trackingandaddictionisbasedonthefactthatFlageletal.(2011)observedthatratsclassi edaseitherSTsorGTsdi erindopaminereleaseduringlearning.InSTstheyobservedanincreaseinCS-evokeddopamineandadecreaseinUS-evokeddopaminerelease.ThispatternwasnotobservedforGTswhenassessingtheirfood-wellentriesbehaviour.However,theyonlyexaminedSTswhiletheyleverpressedandGTswhiletheywereinteractingwiththefood-well.Itwouldbeinterestingtoassesswhetheranincreaseisobservedinsign-trackinganimalswhentheygoal-track,asthiswouldpointtoade nitiveoriginofthesedi erencesintheanimal,notthebehaviourperse.But,aswillbeshowninChapter2,thisissuecanalsobeaddressedatabehaviourallevel.27 Thelinkbetweendopamine,addictionandsign-trackingisgenerallyintermsofsimilaritiesofcertaintypesofbehavioursortraitsassumedtobecharacteristicofad-dictedindividuals(e.g.,impulsivity,in exibility).Itisgenerallybasedontheideathatdopamineisinvolvedinreward-processesandthefactthatindividualsaddictedtoharmfulsubstanceshaveincreasedconcentrationofdopaminereleaseinlimbicre-gions(Volkow,Fowler,Wang,&Swanson,2004).However,thelinkwithsign-trackingbehaviourissomewhatunclearandbasedonexperimentsthatdonotassessbothbe-haviours(sign-andgoal-tracking)inanimalsclassi edasSTsorGTs.Whetherthereisalinkornotbetweenaddictionandsign-trackingbehaviourisnotthecentralissu

ehere,andIpresentedthisapproachbecauseitisrelevantforunder-standingindividualdi erencesinconditionedresponses.Theassumptionforthislineofargumentisthatgeneticindividualdi erencesareresponsiblefortherat'stendencytosignorgoal-track.AstudybyPatituccietal.(2016)challengesthisview.Theypre-sentedtwolevers,onereinforcedwithfoodandanotheronesucrose.Theyfoundthattherat'sbiastowardssign-orgoal-trackingbehaviourdidnotcorrelateacrossthesetwolevers.Inotherwords,itwaspossibleforarattobeaSTononeleverbutnottheother.Di erencesinthebalancebetweensign-andgoal-trackingbehaviouracrosssituations,appearstobeinconsistentwiththerebeingageneraltendencytoonetypeofbehaviourorother,andhencetheideaof`anaddictivepersonality'asrevealedinageneralpropensitytosign-trackingacrosssituationsappearsdiculttosustain.Therearesimilaritiesbetweentheanalysesbasedonthedivisionbetweenmodel-basedandmodel-freeprocessesononehand,andS-SandS-Rprocessesontheother.Model-basedisbasedonthecurrentvalueoftheCS-USrelationshipanditisagoal-directedaction(Beierholm,Anen,Quartz,&Bossaerts,2011).Modelfree,incontrast,isheldtobeafastsystembasedongeneralresponsesorhabitsperformedinanygivensituation.InthesamewayastheS-SandS-Rframework,wecouldassumebehaviourdirectedtowardthefood-wellandleveraregeneratedbyindependentsystems(model-basedormodel-free)thatoperateinparallel.Thisanalysispredictsthatagivenformof28 responsewillexhibitthesamecharacteristicsinaSTsandGTs(e.g.,di erentsensitivitytochangebeingre ectedinthebehaviour).However,insteadofbothsystemsoperatinginparallel,themodelproposedbyLesaintetal.(2014)assumesthatthebehaviourofagivenrodentisgovernedpredominantly

bytheoperationofasinglesystem(eithermodel-basedormodel-free),whichcontrolsbothtypesofbehaviours.Thisaccountpredictsthatsign-andgoal-trackingbehaviourswillexhibitdi erentpropertiesinSTsandGTs.InChapter2IinvestigatethesepredictionsbyexaminingbothbehavioursinSTsandGTsbychangingthecontingencybetweentheCSandUSorthevalueoftheUS.Insummary,thefewattemptstoexplainindividualdi erencesinconditionedre-sponses,ormorespeci callysign-andgoaltrackingbehaviour,haveappealedtoadualmechanismwhichunderpineachofthetwobehaviours.Thereisevidenceconsistentwithadual-systemaccountwhereactionsarecontrolledbyeitherafasthabitualsys-temoraslowermoregoal-directedsystem,basedoncurrentassociationsbetweenstimuli(Beierholmetal.,2011).However,neitherapproachprovidesclearpredictionsofhoweachtypeofbehaviour(sign-orgoal-tracking)willbea ectedbyacertainexperimen-talmanipulation.Moreover,giventhecontinuousnatureofthebalancebetweenthetendencytosign-orgoaltrack(e.g.,Patituccietal.,2016),adual-mechanismaccountwouldneedtoaddresstheinteractionbetweenthetwoprocessesinagivenratorproce-dure.Thisissuehasnotbeendealtwithbyanyofthedual-processesframeworks.Thatsaid,generallearningtheories,whichrelyonasingle-process(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner,1981),areunabletopredictdi erencesinresponding,asperformanceisviewedmerelyasatranslationofassociativestrengthbasedonlearningthecontingencybetweentheCSandUS.1.5ThesisOutlineThisthesisisconcernedwithbetterunderstandingofthenatureofindividualdif-ferencesinPavlovianconditioningandbehaviour.Ipresentnewempiricalevidencein29 Chapter2andChapter4.InChapter2,Iexaminethedi eren

tialsensitivitytocontin-gencychangesinbothSTsandGTs,measuringbothsign-andgoal-trackingbehaviourinbothgroups.Thisevidencecontributedtothedevelopmentofatheoreticalmodel,HeiDI,presentedinChapter3.Themodeldistancesitselffromadualprocessapproachinexplainingperformanceandfromasingle-processperspectivedi erencesand,atthesametime,leadstonovelpredictions.InChapter4,Iexaminetwopredictions.OnepredictionrelatestohowchangesinthevalueoftheUSa ectthedistributionofsign-andgoaltracking.Thesecondpredictionconcernsananalysisofthefeaturepositivee ect,wherethediscriminationemergesmorereadilyforafeaturepositivedesign,wherethecompoundisreinforced(AB+)andtheelementisnon-reinforced(B-),incontrastwithafeaturenegativedesign,wherethecompoundisnon-reinforced(AB-)andtheelementisreinforced(B+).30 Chapter2Investigatingthenatureofphenotypicvari-ation2.1IntroductionPavlovianconditioningisperhapsthemostwell-knownpsychologicalphenomenon,anditstheoreticalimportancewasevidentfromthepointofitsinitialdescription.Theuseofthisparadigmiswidespread,particularlyacrossthe eldsofbehaviouralandcognitiveneuroscience(forarecentreview,seeMurphy&Honey,2016)andbehaviouralgenetics(e.g.,Duvarci,Nader,&LeDoux,2008;Lonsdorfetal.,2009).Inthe eldofbehaviouralneuroscience,oneofitsprincipaluseshasbeeninprovidingatestbedforformaltheoriesofassociativelearning,whichassumethatorganismsformassociationsbetweentherepresentationsofconditionedandunconditionedstimuli(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce,1994;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner,1981).Thesestimulus{stimulus(S-S)associationshavebeencontrastedwiththeformationofstimulus{response(S-R)associationsbetwee

ntheprocessesactivatedbythestimulusandthemotorprogramforgeneratingaresponse(Hull,1943;Spence,1936,1937).Theideathattwo(associative)systemsmightunderpinconditionedbehaviourhasclearcounterpartsincognitiveneuroscienceasdiscussedinChapter1(Reinforcementlearningsection)(e.g.,Daw,Gershman,Seymour,Dayan,&Dolan,2011;Dayan&Berridge,2014).LikePavlov,theformaltheoriesofassociativelearningidenti edaboveappealtotheideathatthememoryorrepresentationofonestimuluscancometoexcite(ortoinhibit)therepresentationofanotherstimulusthroughanexcitatory(orinhibitory)associationformedbetweenthem.UnlikePavlov,however,suchtheorieshaveeschewedconsiderationofindividualdi erencesinPavlovianconditioning,apartfrominsofarastheyrepresentonesourceofvarianceinbehaviouralmeasuresoflearning(seeMatzelet31 al.,2003).Theviewthattherearemarkedindividualdi erencesinsimpleconditioninghasbeenamplycon rmedinmorerecentexperimentswithrodents,wherethedi erencesareperhapsmorestrikingandwellcharacterized.Thesedi erencestoohavepotentialtranslationalsigni cance(seeFlageletal.,2009;Lovicetal.,2011).AsdiscussedinChapter1(Individualdi erencessection),criticalbehaviouralob-servationscomefromsimpleautoshapingprocedures.Inaprocedurewhereratsreceivebriefpresentationsofaleverthatarepairedwiththedeliveryofareinforcerintoafood-well(e.g.,Patituccietal.,2016),markedindividualdi erencesareobservedinbehaviour:someratspredominantlyinteractwiththeleverwhileothersapproachthefood-wellduringtheleverpresentations.Thebasisforthesedi erentphenotypesisthecentralissuethatisaddressedhere.Oneanalysisofindividualdi erencesinsign-andgoal-trackingbehavi

ourcanbede-rivedfromtheassumptionthatthetypesofassociativestructuresdescribedabove(i.e.,S-SorS-R)mightbedi erentlyrepresentedacrossindividuals(seeLesaintetal.,2014;Patituccietal.,2016).Thegeneralideathatacquiredbehavioursmightbetheproductofdi erentsystemswithdistinctcharacteristicshasaclearprecedentinthecontextofstudiesofinstrumentalconditioning(e.g.,Dickinson&Balleine,2002b),andtherearetwosourcesofevidencethatareconsistentwithitfromstudiesofphenotypicdi erencesinPavlovianconditionedresponding.First,food-wellactivityinratsclassi edasgoal-trackers(GTs)declinesmorerapidlyduringanextinctionprocedurethandoesleverpressinginratsclassi edassign-trackers(STs;Ahrensetal.,2016).Theseobservationssuggestthatfood-wellactivityre ectsthecurrentstatusoftherelationshipbetweentheleverandthereinforcer(i.e.,anS-Sassociation),whereaslever-orientedbehaviourwasbasedonaS-Rhabitthatwasmoreresistanttochangesincontingencies.Second,thebiastowardapproachingthefood-wellrelativetoleverpressingispositivelycorre-latedwiththepalatabilityofthereinforcer(Patituccietal.,2016),andsatingrodentswiththereinforcera ectsconditionedfood-wellactivitybutnotlever-orientedactivity(Morrisonetal.,2015;Patituccietal.,2016).32 Theresultsdescribedintheprecedingparagraphareconsistentwiththegeneralideathattherearetwolearningsystemsthatoperatedi erentlyacrossrats;butthereareatleasttwoformsthatthisanalysiscouldtake.Forexample,whilefood-wellbehaviourmightbethedominantresponsegeneratedbytheS-Ssystemandlever-orientedbe-haviourthedominantresponsegeneratedbytheS-Rsystem,bothsystemsmighthavethecapacitytogeneratebothresponses(seeLesaintetal.,2014).

Ifasinglesystemgov-ernedallbehaviourinagivenratthenbothfood-wellandlever-pressresponsesshouldexhibitthecharacteristicpropertyofthatsystem:WhengovernedbyanS-Ssystem,activitydirectedtowardboththeleverandthefood-wellwillchangerapidlyinthefaceofachangeincontingencies;whereaswhengovernedbyanS-Rsystembothwillchangerelativelyslowly.Inprinciple,theaccuracyofthispredictioncouldhavebeenassessedbyAhrensetal.(2016),but,theyonlypresentedactivitydirectedtowardthefood-wellforGTsandtowardtheleverforSTs.Itisnot,therefore,possibletoassesswhetherthetwoformsofresponsewerea ecteddi erentlyinratsclassi edasGTsorSTs.Thesingle-systemanalysisjustoutlinedis,however,challengedbythefollowingobservation:Agivenrodentcanbeclassi edasaGT(orST)withrespecttotheirbehaviouronaleverthatpredictsonereinforcer(e.g.,foodpellets),butnotclassi edinthesamewayonanotherleverthatpredictsadi erentreinforcer(e.g.,sucrose;Patituccietal.,2016,Experiment1).Ifasingle-system(S-SorS-R)governedbehaviourinagivenan-imalthenthepatternsofbehaviourshouldbeconsistentacrossdi erentmanipulanda(i.e.,theleftandrightlevers).Asimplealternativetotheanalysisdescribedinthepreviousparagraphassumesthatbehavioursdirectedtowardthefood-wellandleveraregeneratedbyindependentsystems(S-SandS-R,respectively)thatoperateinparallel.Thisanalysispredictsthatagivenformofresponsewillexhibitthesamecharacteristicsindependentlyofwhethertheanimalinwhichitisobservedisclassi edasaSToraGT;withfood-wellactivitybeingderivedfromtheoperationofaS-Ssystemandlever-orientedbehaviourbeingderivedfromaS-Rsystemthatoperatetodi erentdegreesinallrodents.Thedominantresponsemightbetowar

dthefood-wellinonerodentand33 leverinanother,butinbothratsfood-wellactivityshouldmorerapidlytrackchangesinreinforcementcontingenciesthanshouldlever-orientedactivity.Asalreadynoted,thispredictionwasnotassessedbyAhrensetal.(2016),butPatituccietal.(2016,Experi-ment2)reportedthatsatiationhadamarkede ectonfood-wellactivitywhenthee ectofthismanipulationwasconsideredacrossratsthathadbeenclassi edasGTsorSTs.Thisobservationisconsistentwiththeideathatactivitydirectedtowardthefood-wellandleverhavethesamepropertiesirrespectiveofwhethertheywereexhibitedinSTsorGTs.Tosummarize,tothebestofmyknowledgenobodyhasdirectlyinvestigatedthefollowingsimplequestion:Doesagiventypeofbehaviour(e.g.,leveroriented)havethesameordi erentcharacteristicswhenassessedinSTsandGTs?Here,Iaddressedthisquestionintwoexperiments.Inbothexperiments,ratsreceivedtrainingproce-duresthatshouldallowthetwophenotypestodevelop,andthenthecontingencieswerechanged(e.g.,thereinforcedleverbecamenon-reinforcedandviceversa,orreinforcerischangedfromamoredesirableonetoalessdesirableoneandviceversa).Thechangesinbehavioursdirectedtowardtheleverandfood-wellwerethenassessedasafunctionofwhethertherodentshadbeenclassi edasSTsorGTsattheendofthe rststageoftraining.EvidencefavouringtheclaimthattheS-Ssystemgeneratesfood-wellac-tivityandtheS-Rsystemgenerateslever-orientedbehaviourwouldtaketheformofacompellingdissociation:Morerapidchangesinfood-wellactivitythaninlever-orientedbehaviourinbothST>groups,despitethequitedi erentlevelsofperformanceanticipatedinratsclassi edaspredominantlygoal-trackingorsign-tracking.34 2.2Experiment12.2.1MethodAnimalsandappara

tusSixteenfemaleSpragueDawleyratswereused(suppliedbyCharlesRiver,UK).Theyhadbeensubjectsinabehaviouraltaskinvolvingdrinkingdi erentconcentrationsofsucrose,butwerenavewithrespecttotheapparatusandproceduresusedinExperiment11.Theirmeanadlibitumweightbeforethestartoftheexperimentwas321g(range:280-366g)andtheyweremaintainedatbetween85and95%oftheseweightsbygivingthemrestrictedaccesstofoodattheendofeachday.Theanimalswerehousedingroupsrangingfromtwotofourinstandardcageswithenvironmentalenrichment(smallwood,paperandcardboardtubes)andmaintainedon12-hr/12-hrlight/darkcycle(lightsonat7a.m.).TheresearchwasconductedinaccordancewithHomeOceregulationsundertheAnimal(Scienti cProcedures)Act1986(PPL303243,PIDominicM.Dwyer).Theapparatusconsistedofeightidenticalconditioningboxesmeasuring30Ö24Ö21cm(HÖWÖD;MedAssociates,Georgia,VT).Eachboxwasplacedinasound-attenuatingshellthatincorporatedaventilationfan,whichmaintainedthebackgroundnoiseat68dB(A).Theboxeshadaluminumsidewallsandclearacrylicfrontbackandtop.The oorwasconstructedfrom19steelrods(4.8mmdiameter,16mmapart)andwassituatedaboveastainlesssteeltray.Foodpellets(45mg:suppliedbyMLab:Richmond,IN)weredeliveredtoa oor-levelrecessedfood-well(aperture:5.3Ö5.3cm)inthecentreoftheleftwall.Thefood-wellwasequippedwithinfrareddetectorsthatallowedthepresenceoftheratinthewelltobeautomaticallyrecorded.Asingleresponsewasregisteredwhenthedetectorwasinterrupted(e.g.,whenarat'ssnoutenteredthefood-well).Tworetractablelevers(4.5Ö1.8Ö0.2cm)werelocated3cmtotheleftandright 1Experiment1wasreplicatedaspartofalargerstudyinwhichtheratswerefromthesamesourceasExperiment1,butweremalerathert

hanfemale.TheresultsfromthisreplicationmatchedthoseofExperiment1.Experiment2alsousedmaleratstocon rmthegeneralityoftheresultsfromExperiment1.35 ofthefood-wellandataheightof4.6cmand1.5cmfromtheedgeofthewall.Aleverpresswasrecordedeachoccasionthattheleverwasdepressedby4mmfromitsusualhorizontalrestingposition.MED-PCsoftwarewasusedtoinsertlevers,deliverfoodpellets,andtorecordfood-wellentriesandleverpresses.ProcedureTheratshadtwo24-minpre-trainingsessionswherefoodpelletsweredeliveredonavariable-time(VT)60-sschedule(range:40{80s).Ratsthenreceivedasinglesessionoftrainingoneachofthenext12daysoftraining,whichoccurredatthesametimeofdayforagivenrat(seedesigninTable1).Thesesessionsconsistedof20trialsonwhichtheleftleverwasinsertedfor10sandthenretractedand20trialsonwhichtherightleverwasinsertedfor10sandwasthenwithdrawn.Forhalfoftherats,thereinforcedlever(L1)wastheleftleverandthenon-reinforcedlever(L2)wastherightlever;andfortheotherhalfL1wastherightleverandL2theleftlever.Theorderinwhichtheleftandrightleverswerepresentedwasrandomwiththeconstraintthattherecouldnotbemorethanthreesametypetrialsinsuccession.Ratswereassignedtothegroupsrandomly.Thetrialsweredeliveredonavariable-time(VT)60-sschedule(range:40{80s).Allratsthenreceivedreversaltrainingfor12daysinwhichL1(e.g.,leftlever)wasnon-reinforcedandL2(e.g.,rightlever)wasreinforced.L1denotestheleverthatwasreinforcedduringtrainingandnon-reinforcedduringreversalandL2theleverthatwasnon-reinforcedduringtrainingandreinforcedduringreversal.Theprocedureusedforthereversalstagewasinotherrespectsidenticaltothetrainingstage.DataAnalysisFormanipulatingthedata,datacleaning,statisticalanalysisan

ddatavisualization,IusedopensourcesoftwareR(RStudio,2015)andJASP(JASPTeam,2018).Fordatacleaningandmanipulation,Iused\tidyr"(Wickham&Henry,2018)and\dplyr"36 Table1:DesignoftheExperiment1 Classi cationTrainingReversalNotation STL1+L1-L1(+k-)L2-L2+L2(-k+) GTL1+L1-L1(+k-)L2-L2+L2(-k+) Note:STreferstoasign-trackerratandGTreferstoagoal-trackerrat.L1andL2refertotwolevers(leftandright,counterbalanced).Duringtraining,L1wasreinforced(\+";foodpellet)andL2wasnon-reinforced(\-").DuringthereversalstageL1wasnotreinforcedandL2wasreinforced(foodpellet).Ratswereclassi edasSTorGTonthebasisoftheirbiastowardsleverpressingorenteringthefood-wellduringthe nalblockoftrainingonthereinforcedlever.Rpackages(Wickham,Henry,&Muller,2019).Forstandardhypothesistesting,Iused\ez"Rpackage(Lawrence,2016)andforBayesianstatisticsIusedJASP(JASPTeam,2018).Fordatavisualizationandrepresentation,Iused\ggplot2"(Wickhametal.,2018)Rpackage,and\gridExtra"Rpackages(Auguie&Anotonov,2017).TheanalysisareautomaticallyreportedintextinAPAformatstyleusing\apa"Rpackage(Gromer,2019).Foranalysisofvariance(ANOVA)whenthesphericityassumptionwasviolated,theanalysiswasreportedwithGreenhouse-Geissercorrection.Forpost-hocunplannedcomparisonsBonferronicorrectionwasapplied(thethresholdfordeclaringap-valuesigni cantwasequalto0.05dividedbythenumberofcomparisons).Standardhypothesistestingdoesnotdirectlyassesswhethertheabsenceofasigni -cante ectissucientevidencetoconcludethatthereisnoe ect.Incontrast,Bayesianstatisticsprovidesaratiooftheprobabilityfortheobserveddataunderdi erentmod-els,suchasamodelbasedonthenullhypothesisrelativetoamodelbasedonsom

especi edalternative.TheresultingBayesfactorscanthenbeinterpretedaccordingtotheconventionsuggestedbyRouder,Speckman,Sun,Morey,andIverson(2009),whereaBayesfactorbetween1and3providesanecdotalsupport,afactorbetween3and10suggestssomesupportingevidence,whileafactorbeyond10indicatesstrongevidence.Ihave,therefore,supplementedstandardnull-hypothesisstatisticaltestingwiththepre-37 sentationofequivalentBayesfactors,whennullresultsareoftheoreticalsigni cance.BayesiananalysiswasconductedwithBayesfactorsformaine ectsandinteractionsforfactorialANOVAinthewaydescribedbyRouder,Morey,Speckman,andProvince(2012)andRouder,Morey,Verhagen,Swagman,andWagenmakers(2017).Successivesessionsduringthetrainingandreversalstageswerecombinedinto12Ö2-dayblocks(6Ötraining:T1-T6;and6Öreversal:R1-R6).Attheendofthetrainingphase,theratsweresplitintotwogroups,sign-trackers(ST)andgoal-trackers(GT),basedontheirtendencytoengagewiththeleverandthefood-well.Abiasscorewascalculatedusingthenumberofleverpressesandfood-wellentriesforthereinforcedlever,L1:(Goal-tracking-Sign-tracking)/(Goal-tracking+Sign-tracking).Individualscoresforeachratwascalculatedbasedontherawleverpressesandfood-wellentriesaveragedacrosslasttwodaysoftraining(T6).Atthisstagebothbehavioursareremarkablystableandhavereachedasymptote.Amediansplitwasusedtodivideratsintothosewithhigherscores(groupGTforgoal-trackers)andthosewithlowerscores(groupSTforsign-trackers).Ascorecloseto1indicatesatendencytoengagesolelywiththefood-wellandascorecloseto-1atendencytoengagesolelywiththelever.Withafairlyevendistributionthemediansplitcriterionisaround0(scoresabove0areclassi edasGTandscoresbelow0a

reclassi edasST).ThedistributionofbehaviourisshowninFigure1.Biasscoresabove-.16wereclassi edasGTsandbelow-.09asSTs.Subsequentanalyseswereconductedseparatelyforleverpressesandfood-wellen-tries,withthemainfocusbeingonthetransitionbetweenthe nalblockoftraining(T6)andthe rstblockofreversal(R1).Ialsoconductedcomplementaryanalysesinwhichthenumberofleverpressesandfood-wellentriesweretreatedinacontinuousfashion.MixedANOVAswereconductedseparatelyforleverpressingandfood-wellentrieswithwithin-subjectsfactorsblock(training6levels:T1-T6;transitionblocks2levels:T6-R1;reversal6levels:R1-R6;)andlever(2levels:reinforced\+"vsnon-reinforced\-")andbetween-subjectsfactorclassi cation(2levels:STvsGT).38 Figure1:Distributionofsign-tracking(leverpresses)andgoal-tracking(food-wellentries)behaviourforExperiment1per(10-s)trialduringlastblockoftraining(T6)forL1(+jj-).Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).2.2.2ResultsThemainresultsfromExperiment1areshowninFigure2andinFigure3.Theanalysiswillbeginwithresultsfromthetrainingstage(left-handpanelsofFigure2),beforemovingtothecriticaltransitionbetweentrainingandreversal(identi edbythegreysection),and nallythereversalstageasawhole(right-handpanels).TrainingInspectionoftheresultsfromthe rststageoftraining(left-handsideoftheupperandlowerpanelsofFigure2)suggeststhatastrainingprogressedratsinbothgroups(STandGT)showedmoreleverpressesandfood-wellentriesduringthereinforced39 Figure2:ResultsfromExperiment1:thee ectsofareversalprocedureonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Mean(+SEM)leverpresses(upperpanel)andfood-wellentries(lowerpanel

)per(10-s)trialacrossthetwostages:training(T1{T6)andreversal(R1{R6).Duringtraining,ratsreceivedpresentationsofoneleverpairedwithfoodpellets(L1(+jj-))andnonreinforcedpresentationsofasecondlever(L2(-jj+));ratswereclassi edassign-trackers(STs)andgoal-trackers(GTs)onthebasisoftheirbehaviourduringthe nalblockoftraining(T6).Theythenreceivedareversal:L1non-reinforcedandL2reinforced.Thegreysectionindicatestransitionbetweeninitialtrainingandthereversalofthecontingencies.L1thanthenonreinforcedL2.Thefactthatduringtheinitialtrainingsessionstherewasahigherleveloffood-wellentriesthanleverpressesprobablyre ectstheimpactofthepretrainingsessionsinwhichfoodpelletsweredeliveredintothefood-well.Inanyevent,thediscriminationinvolvingleverpresseswasmoreevidentinGroupSTthanGroupGT,whilethediscriminationinvolvingfood-wellentrieswasmoreevidentinGroupGTthanGroupST,withthesebetween-groupsdi erencesbeingmostapparentonreinforcedL1trials.Thedescriptionofthetrainingresultsissupportedbyseparateanalysesofleverpressesandfood-wellentries.40 Figure3:CorrelationresultsfromExperiment1:thee ectsofareversalprocedureonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Theupperpanelsshowtherelationshipbetweenthemeannumberofresponsesper(10-s)trialbetweenblocksT5andT6forleverpresses(A)andforfood-wellentries(B)onreinforcedtrials.ThelowerpanelsshowtherelationshipT6andR1forleverpresses(C)andfood-wellentries(D)onreinforcedtrials.Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).LeverpressesAnANOVAconductedforleverpressesrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=6.36,p=.024,2p=.31,block,F(2.96,41.41)=9.74,p.001,2p=.41

andlever,F(1,14)=40.06,p.001,2p=.74,aswellasinteractionsbetweenclas-si cationandblock,F(2.96,41.41)=2.15,p=.109,2p=.13,classi cationandleverF(1,14)=6.88,p=.020,2p=.33,andblockandleverF(2.60,36.35)=18.75,p.001,2p=.57.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.60,36.35)=3.42,p=.033,2p=.20.41 Food-wellentriesAparallelanalysiswasconductedforfood-well-entriesandrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cationF(1,14)=12.19,p=.004,2p=.47,andlever,F(1,14)=37.82,p.001,2p=.73,butnotablocke ect,F(2.68,37.49)=11.98,p.001,2p=.46.Therewereinteractionsbetweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,14)=12.90,p=.003,2p=.48,classi cationandblock,F(2.68,37.49)=7.30,p.001,2p=.34andbetweenblockandleverF(2.83,39.69)=7.40,p.001,2p=.35.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.83,39.69)=10.33,p.001,2p=.42.TransitionblocksTheresultsfromthetransitionbetweenthe nalblockoftrainingand rstblockofreversal(graysectionofFigure2)areofcentralinterest.Inspectionofthistransi-tionhighlightsthefactthatleverpressesremainedstableinspiteofthereversedrein-forcementcontingencies(upperpanel),whereasfood-wellentrieschangedrapidly(lowerpanel).Moreover,thesedi erencesbetweenthee ectsofthereversalonleverpressesandfood-wellentrieswereevidentinbothGroupsSTandGT:Thelevelsofleverpress-ingremainedlargelyunchangedinbothgroups;andwhiletherewasamarkeddecreaseinfood-wellentriestothepreviouslyreinforcedleverinGroupGTthereweremarkedincreasesinfood-wellentriestothepreviouslynonreinforcedleverinbothGroupGTandST.Also,inGroupSTtherewasamoremarkedincr

easeinrespondingbetweentheT6andR1forL2thanL1.LeverpressesAnANOVAconductedforleverpressesonthecriticalreversalblocksrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=7.71,p=.015,2p=.36andlever,F(1,14)=55.27,p.001,2p=.80,butnoe ectofblock,F(1,14)=0.82,p=.380,2p=.06.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,14)=6.90,p=.020,2p=.33,butnointeractionbetweenclassi cationandblockF(1,14)=0.07,p=.800,2p.01,andcriticallynointeractionbetweenblockandlever,F(1,14)=1.68,p=.216,2p=.11.42 Therewasnotripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandleverF(1,14)=0.13,p=.728,2p.01.TheBayesfactorforthebestmodelwithouttheinteractionbetweenblockandleverrelativetothemodelwiththeinteractionwas9.52,whichindicatesevidenceagainstthepresenceoftheinteraction.TheBayesfactorforthebestmodelwithouttheinteractionbetweenblock,leverandgrouprelativetothemodelwiththeinteractionwas62.50,whichrepresentsstrongevidenceagainstthepresenceoftheinteraction.Thisanalysisindicatesthattherewerenoimmediatee ectsofthereversalonleverpressesineitherofgroupsSTandGT.Food-wellentriesAparallelanalysisoffood-wellentriesforthecriticaltransitionblocksrevealedane ectofclassi cation,F(1,14)=18.90,p.001,2p=.57,blockF(1,14)=12.04,p=.004,2p=.46andlever,F(1,14)=11.88,p=.004,2p=.46.Therewereinteractionsbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(1,14)=9.68,p=.008,2p=.41,classi cationandlever,F(1,14)=10.75,p=.005,2p=.43andmoreimportantlyaninteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(1,14)=43.68,p.001,2p=.76.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(1,14)=11.87,p=.004,2p

=.46.Theanalysisjustpresentedinvolveddividingratsintotwogroups(STandGT)usingtheirbiasesduringthe nalblockoftraining.However,thesameconclusionsaresupportedbyananalysisinwhichtheirleverpressesandfood-wellentriesaretreatedasacontinuum.TheupperpanelsofFigure3depicttherelationshipbetweenleverpresses(panelA)onreinforcedL1trialsforthe nalblocksoftraining(i.e.,T5andT6;left-handpanel),andbetweenfood-wellentriesonL1trialsforthesameblocks(panelB).Thelowerpanelsdepicttherelationshipsbetweenleverpressesonthe nalblockoftrainingandthe rstblockofreversal(i.e.,T6andR1,panelC)onL1trials,andbetweenfood-wellentriesforthesametwoblocks(panelD)onL1trials.Thegroupmembershipofeachratisidenti ed.Forbothtypesofresponse,therewasasigni cantcorrelationbetweenT5andT6,leverpresses,(r(14)=.94,p.001;food-wellentries,r(14)=.93,p.001).However,whiletherewasacorrelationbetweenT6andR1for43 leverpresses(r(14)=.83,p.001,therewasnotforfood-wellentries(r(14)=-.25,p=.355).Food-wellentrieschangedbetweenT6andR1,butleverpressesdidnot.ReversalAcrosstheblocksofreversaltraining(right-handsideoftheupperandlowerpanelsofFigure2),thenumbersofleverpressesincreasedduringL2anddecreasedduringL1inGroupST,andthischangewasnumericallysmallerinGroupGT.Incontrast,thenumberoffood-wellentriesincreasedduringL2anddecreasedinL1inGroupGT,andthischangewaslessapparentinGroupST.LeverpressesAnANOVAforleverpressesfortheblocksfollowingthereversalrevealedmaine ectsofblock,F(2.95,41.24)=17.07,p.001,2p=.55,butnoe ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=3.60,p=.079,2p=.20orlever,F(1,14)=1.03,p=.327,2p=.07.Therewasablockbyleverinteraction,F(1.68,23.58)=2

1.96,p.001,2p=.61,butnointeractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.95,41.24)=2.56,p=.069,2p=.15,orclassi cationandlever,F(1,14)=0.01,p=.919,2p.01.Therewasnosigni cantinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(1.68,23.58)=3.48,p=.054,2p=.20.Food-wellentriesAparallelanalysisforfood-wellentriesrevealedamaine ectoflever,F(1,14)=22.76,p.001,2p=.62,butnoe ectofclassi cation,F(1,14)=0.13,p=.721,2p.01,orblock,F(1.70,23.75)=1.42,p=.259,2p=.09.TherewasablockbyleverinteractionF(1.85,25.90)=13.04,p.001,2p=.48,butnointeractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(1.70,23.75)=0.46,p=.604,2p=.03,orclassi -cationandlever,F(1,14)=2.36,p=.147,2p=.14.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(1.85,25.90)=3.67,p=.042,2p=.21.44 2.2.3DiscussionExperiment1Discriminationtrainingwherethepresentationofonelever(L1)waspairedwithfoodpelletsandanother(L2)wasnot,resultedinmarkedindividualdi erencesinconditionedresponding;withsomeratsinteractingwithL1(butnotL2)andothersapproachingthesiteoffooddeliveryduringL1(butnotL2).Whenthecontingencieswerereversed,withL1nownon-reinforcedandL2reinforced,thedi erentlevelsofleverpressingtoL1(andL2)inGroupsSTandGTremainedremarkablystableduringthe rstblockofreversal.Incontrast,thelevelsoffood-wellentrieschangedmorerapidlyinbothGroupsSTandGT(Figure2).Thisdi erentialsensitivityofthetworesponseformstochangingcontingencieswasalsoevidentwhentheywereconsideredascontinuousvariables(Figure3).Theseresultsdemonstratethatthedissociationbetweenleverpresses(inratsdesignatedasSTs)andfood-wellentries

(inratsdesignatedasGTs)doesnotre ectadi erenceinthesensitivityofthetwogroupstochangedreinforcementcontingenciesperse(cf.Ahrensetal.,2016).Instead,theseresultsshowthattheleverpressandfood-wellentryresponsesaredi erentlysensitivetosuchchangesirrespectiveofthephenotypeoftherat.Theseobservationssuggestthatthedistinctbehavioursre ecttheparalleloperationofS-SandS-Rsystemswithinanindividual,ratherthantheoperationofasinglesystem(eitherS-SorS-R)thatgivesrisetobothbehaviours(cf.Lesaintetal.,2014).Experiment2attemptedtoextendtheseobservationsbyexaminingwhetherchangesinthenatureofthereinforcer(betweenalternativesthatproducedi erentlevelsofresponding)producemorerapidchangesinfood-wellactivitythaninleverpressinginSTsandGTs.2.3Experiment2InExperiment1,IexaminedhowtheextinctionofanassociationbetweenaleverandanappetitiveUSa ectedbehaviouronleverpressesandfood-wellentriesinbothSTsandGTs.InExperiment2,ratsreceivedseparatepresentationsoftwolevers(L145 andL2)thatwerebothpairedwiththesamereinforcerduringtraining(eitherfoodpelletsorsucrose).Pilotresearchhadestablishedthatfoodpelletsmaintainhigherlevelsofbothleverpressingandfood-wellentriesthandoessucrose(seealsoPatituccietal.,2016)whichshouldbeevidentinthe rststageoftraininginExperiment2.Theratsthatwerereinforcedwithpelletsandsucrosewerefurtherdividedintotwogroups(GroupSTadGT)onthebasisoftheirbiasesattheendoftraining.Duringthesecondstage,thereinforcersassociatedwiththetwoleverswereswitched:theratsgivenpelletsduringtrainingreceivedsucroseduringtheswitchandthosegivensucroseduringtrainingreceivedfoodpelletsduringtheswitch(seedesigninTable2).Theissueofcentralinterestwas

theextenttowhichthetwotargetbehavioursthathaddevelopedduringL1andL2(leverpressesandfood-wellentries)wouldchangetore ectthefactthattheleverswerenowpairedwithreinforcersthatmaintaineddi erentlevelsofperformance(i.e.,foodpelletsandsucrose).IfthebehaviourofratsinGroupSTisgeneratedbyaS-Rsystem,thenbothleverpressesandfood-wellentriesshouldbelesssensitivetothechangeinreinforcertypethanthoseinGroupGT,whosebehaviourisgeneratedbyaS-Ssystem.However,ifleverpressingisbasedonaS-Rsystemwhereasfood-wellentriesre ectaS-Ssystem,thenleverpressingshouldbelesssensitivetothechangeincontingenciesbetweenthetrainingandswitchstagesthanshouldfood-wellentries,irrespectiveofwhetherthosebehavioursareexpressedinGroupSTorGroupGT.2.3.1MethodAnimalsandapparatusThirty-twonavemaleListerHoodedrats(suppliedbyEnvigo,Bicester,U.K.)werehousedinthesamewayasdescribedinExperiment1.Theirmeanadlibitumweightwas295g(range284{320g).Ratshadfreeaccesstowater,andtheyweremaintainedbetween85and95%oftheiradlibweightsbygivingthemrestrictedaccesstofoodattheendofeachday.TheexperimentalchamberswerethoseusedinExperiment46 1.ThedesignoftheexperimentissimilartoExperiment1,withtheadditionthatinExperiment2,thesucrosedipperdelivered0.05mlofsucrosesolution(8%weight/weightwithwater)forhalftherats(seeTable2).Thesucrosedipperwassituatedattheright-handofthefood-wellincloseproximityofthepelletdispenser.Thedefaultpositionofthedipperislevelledupandaccessibletoratsandatpointofrewarddeliverythedipperlowersandgetsre lled.Thatis,sucroseremainsavailableinde nitely,likepellets,butonlytheamountdeliveredbyoneloweringofthedipper.Respondingwasrecordedinthesamewayasdescribedfor

foodpelletsinExperiment1.ProcedureTheratshadtwo24-minpretrainingsessionsbeforethetrainingandswitchstages.Duringthesesessions,theratsreceivedthereinforcer(foodpelletsorsucrose)thatwastobedeliveredintheimmediatelysucceedingstage.Thereinforcersweredeliveredonavariable60-sschedule(range40{80s).Ratsreceived12daysoftrainingthatwerear-rangedinthesamewayasExperiment1withtheexceptionthatthepresentationofbothlevers(L1andL2-leftandrightlevers,counterbalanced)werefollowedbyareinforcer(foodpelletsforhalfoftheratsandsucrosefortheremainder).Theswitchstagealsoconsistedof12days.Thisstagewasidenticaltothetrainingstagewiththeexceptionthattheratsthathadreceivedfoodpelletsduringthetrainingstagereceivedsucroseduringtheswitchstage,andthosethathadreceivedsucroseduringtrainingreceivedfoodpelletsduringtheswitch.Ratswererandomlyassignedtotheexperimentalgroups.DataAnalysisTherewerestrongpositivecorrelationsbetweenleverpressbehaviourduringthepre-sentationsofL1andL2thatwerebothpairedwiththesameoutcome(eitherfoodpelletsorsucrose)andbetweenfood-wellbehaviouronthetwolevers.Theseobservationshavesometheoreticalsigni cancewhencontrastedwiththeresultsofPatituccietal.(2016),47 Table2:DesignofExperiment2 Classi cationTrainingSwitchNotation STL1&L2PelL1&L2SucSTPelkSucororL1&L2SucL1&L2PelSTSuckPel GTL1&L2PelL1&L2SucGTPelkSucororL1&L2SucL1&L2PelGTSuckPel Note:STreferstoasign-trackerratandGTreferstoagoal-trackerrat.L1andL2refertotwolevers(leftandright,counterbalanced).Duringtraining,bothleverswerepairedwithonereinforcer(foodpelletsorsucrose),andduringtheswitch,bothleverswerethenpairedwiththeotherreinforcer(sucroseorfoodpellets,respectively).Ratswe

reclassi edasSTorGTonthebasisoftheirbiastowardsleverpressingorenteringthefood-wellduringthe nalblockoftraining.whoreportednocorrelationbetweenthesign-andgoal-trackingbiasesontwoleversthatsignalleddi erentoutcomes.IshallconsidertheimplicationsofthisevidenceintheChapter2Discussion.However,tosimplifytheresultssection,theprincipalanalysisoftheresultsofExperiment2willbeconductedwiththefrequencyofresponsescombinedacrosstheleftandrightlevers.AsinExperiment1,thetrainingandswitchsessionswerecombinedinto2-dayblocksforthepurposeofanalysis.Theratsweresplitintotwogroups,STsandGTs,usingthebiasscoredescribedinExperiment1.Thesplitwasconductedseparatelyforthesubgroupsofratsthatreceivedfoodpelletsandsucroseduringthetrainingstage.Thisresultedinfourgroups(8ineachgroup):STPelkSuc(pelletsduringtraining,sucroseduringtheswitchstage),GTPelkSuc(GTs;pelletsduringtraining,sucroseduringtheswitch),STSuckPel(STs;sucroseduringtraining,pelletsduringtheswitch)andGTSuckPel(GTs;sucroseduringtraining,pelletsdur-ingtheswitch).ForthePelkSucgroupbiasscoresabove.33wereclassi edasGTsandscoresbelow.31asSTs.FortheSuckPelgroupscoresabove.58wereclassi edasGTsandbelow.54asSTs.Thedistributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourisshowninFigure4.48 MixedANOVAswereconductedseparatelyforleverpressingandfood-wellentrieswithwithin-subjectsfactorblock(forthetrainingandtheswitchstage6levels:T1-T6andS1-S6;transitionstage2levels:T6-S1),andbetween-subjectsfactorsreinforcer(2levels:pelvssuc)andclassi cation(2levels:STvsGT). Figure4:Distributionofsign-tracking(leverpresses)andgoal-tracking(food-wellentries)behaviourforExperiment2per(10-s)trialdu

ringlastblockoftraining(T6)averagedacrossL1andL2.Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).2.3.2ResultsThemainresultsfromExperiment2areshowninFigure5andinFigure6.AsinExperiment1,theanalysisoftheresultsofExperiment2willbeginwithresultsfromthetrainingstage,beforemovingtoacomparisonofthe nalblockoftrainingwiththe rstblockofreversal(identi edbythegreysection),and nallytheswitchstageasawhole.49 Figure5:ResultsfromExperiment2:thee ectsofaswitchinUSvalueonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Mean(SEM)leverpresses(upperpanel)andfood-wellentries(lowerpanel)per(10-s)trialacrossthetwostages:training(T1{T6)andswitch(S1{S6).Duringtraining,ratsreceivedpresentationsoftwoleverspairedwitheitherpelletsorsucrose.Ratswereclassi edassign-trackers(ST)andgoal-trackers(GT)onthebasisoftheirbehaviourduringthe nalblockoftraining(T6).Thereinforcersthatfollowedtheleverswereswappedduringthesecondswitchstage.Thegreysectionindicatestransitionbetweeninitialtrainingandtheswapfromsucrosetofoodpelletrewards(orfrompelletstosucrose).Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).Solidlinesindicategroupsreceivingpelletsinphase1andsucroseaftertheswitchtophase2(PelkSuc)anddashedlinesthoseswitchedfromsucrosetopellets(SuckPel).Forexample,themeansfortheSTgroupthatreceivedfoodpelletsduringtrainingandsucroseduringtheswitcharegivenbyblacksymbols(ST)andcontinousline(PelkSuc).TrainingInspectionoftheleft-handsideoftheupperandlowerpanelsinFigure5suggeststhattheSTgroupsaremorelikelytoengageinleverpressingthanaretheGTgroups,andthattheGTgroupsaremorelikelytoenterthefood-w

ellthantheSTgroups.Thesegroupdi erences,especiallyinthecaseoffood-wellactivity,weremostmarkedwhenfoodpelletswerethereinforcer.Thisdescriptionofthetrainingresultspresented50 Figure6:CorrelationresultsfromExperiment2:thee ectsofaswitchinUSvalueonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Theupperpanelsshowtherelationshipbetweenthemeannumberofresponsesper(10-s)trialbetweenblocksT5andT6forleverpresses(A)andforfood-wellentries(B)onreinforcedtrials.ThelowerpanelsshowtherelationshipT6andR1forleverpresses(C)andfood-wellentries(D)onreinforcedtrials.Theresponse(leverpressingandfood-wellentries)hasbeenpooledacrossL1andL2trials.Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).inFigure5issupportedbyseparateanalysesofleverpressesandfood-wellentries.LeverpressesANOVAconductedonleverpresses,pooledacrossthetwolevers,con rmedthereweremaine ectsofreinforcer,F(1,28)=31.02,p.001,2p=.53,classi -cation(STvsGT),F(1,28)=9.66,p=.004,2p=.26,andblock(T1-T6),F(3.15,88.33)=24.96,p.001,2p=.47.Therewereinteractionsbetweenrein-forcerandblock,F(3.15,88.33)=7.76,p.001,2p=.22,andbetweenclassi cation51 andblock,F(3.15,88.33)=7.11,p.001,2p=.20,butnotbetweenreinforcerandclassi cation,F(1,28)=2.76,p=.108,2p=.09.Thethree-wayinteractionbetweenreinforcer,classi cationandblockwasnotsigni cant,F(3.15,88.33)=1.06,p=.373,2p=.04.Food-wellentriesAparallelanalysisoffood-wellentriesrevealedmaine ectsofreinforcer,F(1,28)=9.86,p=.004,2p=.26,classi cation,F(1,28)=10.47,p=.003,2p=.27andblock,F(2.81,78.57)=19.48,p.001,2p=.41.Therewerealsointeractionsbetwee

nreinforcerandclassi cation,F(1,28)=5.42,p=.027,2p=.16,andbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.81,78.57)=8.65,p.001,2p=.24.Therewasnointer-actionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.81,78.57)=2.14,p=.105,2p=.07,andnotripleinteractionbetweenreinforcer,classi cationandblock,F(2.81,78.57)=2.64,p=.059,2p=.09.TransitionblocksInspectionofthegreypanelinFigure5showsthattherewererapidchangesinfood-wellentries(lowerpanel)butnotinleverpressing(upperpanel).Tobemorespeci c:Thehighleveloffood-wellactivity-previouslymaintainedbypellets-declinedfromT6toS1,andthelowleveloffood-wellactivity-previouslymaintainedbysucrose-increasedfromT6toS1.Incontrast,leverpressingwaslargelyunchangedacrossT6andS1.Thisdescriptionwassupportedbyseparateanalysisofleverpressesandfood-wellentries.LeverpressesANOVAconductedforleverpressesrevealedane ectofreinforcer,F(1,28)=33.57,p.001,2p=.55andclassi cation,F(1,28)=14.72,p.001,2p=.34,butnoe ectofblock,F(1,28)=1.86,p=.183,2p=.06.Therewasaninteractionbetweenreinforcerandclassi cation,F(1,28)=4.74,p=.038,2p=.14,andaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(1,28)=4.70,p=.039,2p=.14.Critically,therewasnointeractionbetweenreinforcerandblock,F(1,28)=0.00,p=.952,2p.01,and52 nothree-wayinteractionbetweenreinforcer,classi cationandblock,F(1,28)=0.23,p=.637,2p.01.TheBayesfactorforthebestmodelwithouttheblockbyleverinteractionrelativetothebestmodelwiththeinteractionis5.88,indicatingevidenceagainstthepresenceoftheinteraction.TheBayesfactorforthebestmodelwithouttheblockbyleverbygroupinteractionrelativetothemodelwiththeinteraction

is100,indicatingstrongevidenceagainstthepresenceoftheinteraction.TheswitchinreinforcerhadlittleimpactonleverpressbehaviourineithertheSTorGTgroups.Food-wellentriesAparallelanalysisoffood-wellentriesrevealedamaine ectofclassi cation,F(1,28)=15.59,p.001,2p=.36andblock,F(1,28)=4.18,p=.050,2p=.13,butnoe ectofreinforcer,F(1,28)=1.22,p=.278,2p=.04.Critically,therewasaninteractionbetweenreinforcerandblock,F(1,28)=95.42,p.001,2p=.77,aswellasbetweenclasi cationandblock,F(1,28)=5.76,p=.023,2p=.17,butnotbetweenreinforcerandclassi cation,F(1,28)=1.94,p=.174,2p=.06,andnotripleinterac-tionbetweenreinforcer,classi cationandblock,F(1,28)=2.14,p=.155,2p=.07.TheBayesfactorforthebestmodelwithouttheinteractionbetweenblock,leverandgrouprelativetothemodelwiththeinteractionis25,indicatingstrongevidenceagainsttheinteraction.Thatistheswitchinreinforcershadanimmediateimpactonbehaviourdirectedtothefood-well,andcriticallythiswasequivalentinboththeSTandGTgroups.Thesameconclusionsaresupportedbyananalysisinwhichleverpressesandfood-wellentriesweretreatedasacontinuum.TheupperpanelsofFigure6depicttherelationshipbetweenleverpressesonthe nalblocksoftraining(i.e.,T5andT6;left-handpanel)andbetweenfood-wellentriesonthesameblocks(right-handpanel)pooledacrossleftandrightlevertrials.Thelowerpanelsdepicttherelationshipsbetweenleverpressesonthe nalblockoftrainingandthe rstblockofswitch(i.e.,T6andS1left-handpanel),andbetweenfood-wellentriesonthesametwoblocks(right-handpanel).Forbothtypesofresponsetherewasasigni cantcorrelationbetweenT5andT6(lever,53 r(30)=.92,p.001;food-wellentries,r(

30)=.81,p.001).BetweenT6andS1thereisasigni cantcorrelationforleverpresses(r(30)=.83,p.001),butnotforfood-wellentries(r(30)=.06,p=.734).SwitchThepatternofresultsevidentonthe rstblockoftheswitch(i.e.,S1)was,forthemostpart,evidentacrossthelaterblocksoftheswitchstage.Morespeci cally,themarkedchangesinfood-wellentriesweresustainedacrosstheswitchstageandwereaccompaniedbylittlechangeinleverpressing:whilethelowlevelofleverpressingincreasedwhensucrosewasreplacedwithfoodpelletsduringtheswitch,thehighlevelofleverpressingwasmaintainedwhenfoodpelletswerereplacedwithsucrose.LeverpressesANOVAconductedonleverpresses,pooledacrossthetwolevers,con rmedtherewasamaine ectofclassi cation,F(1,28)=13.69,p.001,2p=.33,howevertherewerenoe ectsofreinforcer,F(1,28)=3.35,p=.078,2p=.11orblock,F(3.51,98.17)=2.20,p=.083,2p=.07.Therewereinteractionsbetweenrein-forcerandblock,F(3.51,98.17)=5.63,p.001,2p=.17,andbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(3.51,98.17)=4.52,p=.003,2p=.14,butnotbetweenreinforcerandclassi cation,F(1,28)=0.68,p=.417,2p=.02.Thethree-wayinteractionbetweenreinforcer,classi cationandblockwasnotsigni cant,F(3.51,98.17)=1.98,p=.112,2p=.07.Food-wellentriesAparallelanalysisoffood-wellentriesrevealedmaine ectsofreinforcer,F(1,28)=20.46,p.001,2p=.42,classi cation,F(1,28)=6.90,p=.014,2p=.20,andblockF(2.87,80.25)=3.59,p=.019,2p=.11.Therewasaninteractionbetweenreinforcerandblock,F(2.87,80.25)=5.35,p=.002,2p=.16,butnointeractionsbetweenreinforcerandclassi cation,F(1,28)=0.03,p=.862,2p.01,orbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.87,80.2

5)=0.82,p=.481,2p=.03,andnotriplein-54 teractionbetweenreinforcer,classi cationandblock,F(2.87,80.25)=1.41,p=.247,2p=.05.2.3.3DiscussionExperiment2TheresultsofExperiment2con rmtheprincipalconclusionsderivedfromtheresultsofExperiment1.First,lever-pressbehaviourwaslesssensitivetochangesinreinforce-mentcontingenciesthanwasfood-wellbehaviour.Second,thisdi erenceinsensitivitywasequallyapparentinratsthatwereclassi edasSTsandGTs.InExperiment1,theseconclusionsweresupportedbythee ectsofareversalbetweentherelationshipsbetweentwolevers(L1andL2)andthepresenceandabsenceoffoodpellets,whereasinExperiment2theyweresupportedbythesubstitutionofreinforcersthatmaintainedmore(pellets)orless(sucrose)behaviour.ThefactsthatExperiment2usedmaleratswhileExperiment1usedfemalerats(anditsresultshavebeenreplicatedinmalerats)andthetwoexperimentsuseddi erentstrains(Sprague-DawleyandListerHooded,respectively),suggeststhatthedi erenceinsensitivityofleverandfood-welldirectedbehaviourtochangesinreinforcementcontingenciesispreservedacrossratstrainsandmale/femaleanimals.2.4Chapter2DiscussionDuringappetitivePavlovianconditioning,rodentswillreliablydisplaybehaviourdirectedbothtowardthestimulus(sign-tracking)andtowardthesiteoffoodpelletdelivery(goal-tracking).Althoughindividualdi erencesinconditionedrespondinghavetypicallyreceivedscantconsiderationintheoriesofassociativelearning,itisclearthatthedistributionofthesebehavioursdi ersacrossindividuals(e.g.,Fitzpatricketal.,2013).Forexample,whenaleveristemporarilyinsertedintoaconditioningchamberandpairedwithfoodpelletssomeratsdevelopaconsistenttendencytointeractwith

theleverwhereasothersdevelopatendencytoapproachthefood-well.Thesebehavioursaredi erentlysensitivetothecurrentvalueofthereinforcerandindeeditspresence.55 Patituccietal.(2016)demonstratedthatthebiastowardengaginginfood-wellactivityratherthanlever-pressactivitywaspositivelycorrelatedwiththepalatabilityofthereinforcer;andsatingratsonthereinforcerreducedfood-wellbutnotlever-orientedactivity;andAhrensetal.(2016)showedthatleverpressing,inratsthatpredominantlyengagedinsign-tracking,waslesssensitivetoextinctionthanfood-wellactivity,inratsthatpredominantlyengagedingoal-tracking.Myresultscon rmthatlever-pressbehaviourisindeedlesssensitivetochangesinreinforcementcontingenciesthanisfood-wellbehaviour.InExperiment1,thiswasevidentinthee ectsofareversalintherelationshipsbetweentwoleversandthepres-enceandabsenceoffoodpellets,whereasinExperiment2itwasevidentinthee ectsofthesubstitutionofreinforcersthatmaintainedmore(pellets)orless(sucrose)be-haviour.Moreover,inbothexperiments,theseconclusionsreceivedadditionalsupportfromtreatinglever-pressandfood-wellactivityinacontinuousway:lever-pressactivitywascorrelatedbetweenthe nalblockoftraining(T6)andthe rstblockofthechangedcontingencies(R1inExperiment1andS1inExperiment2),butfood-wellactivitywasnot.Here,Icontrastedtwopossibleaccountsofthebehaviouralphenotypes,basedontheassumptionthatsign-andgoal-trackingaremodulatedbytwosystems(e.g.,S-S/S-R,ormodel-based/model-free).First,thatthebehaviourofagivenrodentisgovernedbytheoperationofasinglesystemandthatthecontrolofbothtypesofbehavioursimplyre ectsthenatureofthegoverningsystem(i.e.,inagoal-trackingratthepredominantsystemth

atdrivesbehaviourisS-Sormodel-based,andinasign-trackingratisS-Rormodel-free).Thisaccountpredictsthatfood-wellandlever-orientedbehaviourswillexhibitdi erentpropertiesinSTsandGTs.Second,thatbehaviourdirectedtowardthefood-wellandleveraregeneratedbyindependentsystems(S-S/model-basedandS-R/model-free,respectively)thatoperateinparallel.ThisanalysispredictsthatagivenformofresponsewillexhibitthesamecharacteristicsinSTsandGTs.InExperiments1and2,lever-pressandfood-wellbehaviourinbothgoal-trackingandsign-trackingratsshowedthesamepatternofsensitivitytochangesinreinforcercontingencies.Thispat-56 ternofresultsprovidessupportforthesecondoftheseaccounts:di erentialsensitivitytocontingencychangesisapropertyofthebehaviour,nottheanimal.WhiletheresultsthatIhavepresentedsofarhaveclearimplicationsregardingthecontrolofbehavioursinthetwobehaviouralphenotypestheydonotcontributetoourunderstandingoftheoriginofthetwophenotypes.Patituccietal.(2016)arguedthatfood-wellactivitywasmorelikelytodominatein(goal-tracking)ratsthat,forwhateverreason,valuedthereinforcermore,basedonthefactthatgoal-trackingcorrelatedwiththepalatability(preference)ofthereinforcer.But,theyalsoobservedthattheclassi- cationofarataseitheraGT(orST)onaleverthatwaspairedwithonereinforcer(e.g.,foodpellets)wasunrelatedtotheclassi cationofthesameratonasecondleverthatwaspairedwithadi erentreinforcer(e.g.,sucrose).Theyarguedthatifagivenratvaluedonereinforcer(e.g.,foodpellets)morethantheother(e.g.,sucrose)thenthiswouldresultinmoregoal-trackingononeleverthananother.Whilethisanalysisiscertainlyconsistentwithotherfeaturesoftheirresults,amoreprosaicaccountcanbedevelope

dforthelackofcorrelationsbetweenthebehavioursdirectedtotwolevers:Itmighthavere ectedsuperstitiousreinforcementofdi erentbehaviours(e.g.,leverorfood-welloriented)thathappenedtooccurduringthetwotypesoftrials.However,inthecurrentExperiment2,thetwoleverswerebothpairedwiththesamereinforcer(foodpelletsorsucrose),andwhilethisnecessarilymeansthatthereisnodi erenceinthevalueofthereinforcerthatispairedwiththelevers,itremainspossiblethatratswillbeengagingindi erentbehavioursduringthetwoleversthatwouldbesubjecttosuperstitiousreinforcement.TheresultsofExperiment2providesupportfortheexplanationpreferredbyPatituccietal.(2016):Whentheleverswerepairedwiththesamereinforcerthereweresigni cantcorrelationsbetweenfood-wellactivityontheleftandrightleversonBlock6(r(30)=.94,p.001),andbetweenlever-pressactivityonthetwoleversduringthesameblock(r(30)=.74,p.001).Thefactthattheextenttowhichphenotypicvariationinsign-andgoal-trackingbehavioursisconsistentacrossleversdependsonwhethertheyarepairedwiththesameordi erentoutcomessuggests57 thatoutcomevaluecontributestoresponseselection.Tosummarize,theresultspresentedinthischapterindicatethatindividualdi er-encesinthetopographyofconditionedbehaviourre ectthebehaviourthatisbeingmeasuredanditsproperties.Theobserveddi erencesre ecttheoperationofdistinctassociativeprocessesthatdi erintheirsensitivitytorewardvalueandchangesincon-tingencies,thisbeingapropertyofthebehaviourandnottheanimal,perse.AsIalreadymentionedintheintroduction,adual-processaccountisabletoexplainsomeresultswhereindividualdi erencesareobserved(e.g.,di erentialsensitivitytochange)butdoesnotprovideasat

isfactoryexplanationoftheoriginofthesedi erences.TheevidencesuggeststhatS-Slearningisnecessaryforacquisitionofsign-andgoal-tracking,buttheroleofS-Rlearningislessclear.Sign-trackingdisplaysthepropertiesofanhabitualsystemgiventheresistancetochange,howeverwehaveseenthatanassociationbetweentheCSandaresponseisnotnecessaryforthebehaviourtoemerge(e.g.,Browne,1976;Williams&Williams,1969).Theseresultsmotivatedthedevelopmentofanewformalmodeloflearningandperformance,HeiDI(describedinChapter3),inwhichtheassociativestructuresthatareacquiredduringPavlovianconditioningareintegratedwithananalysisofhowtheknowledgeembodiedinthesestructuresdeterminesthenatureoftheresponseselicitedbyaCS.Neitherofthedual-processaccountsisabletoexplaintheoriginoftheobservedindividualdi erencesortopredictwhenorhowthebalancefavoursonesystemortheother.HeiDI,echoestheconclusionspresentedinChapter2andatthesametimedistancesitselffromthedual-processaccountinexplainingconditionedbehaviour.ThemodelisexplainedindetailthefollowingChapter.58 Chapter3HeiDI:AmodelforPavlovianlearningandperformancewithreciprocalassociationsHeidi,oneoftheworld'smostpopularchildren'sstories,wasoriginallywrittenbyJohannaSpyriastwocompanionpieces:Heidi:Heryearsofwanderingandlearning,andHeidi:Howsheusedwhatshelearned.TheydescribehowHeidi'spredispositiontowanderandlearnwaslaterevidentinherbehaviour.ThecentralconcernofthemodeldevelopedhereisthenatureoftheassociativestructuresthatareacquiredduringPavlovianconditioningandhowthesestructuresresultintheirbehaviouralsequelae.Pavlovianconditioningisprobablythebest-knownphenomenoninthehistoryofthescienti cstudyofpsychology.Thebasicproc

edureandobservationscanberecountedbypeoplewithlittleornootherknowledgeofthe eld:dogsgivenpairingsofaringingbellwithfoodcometosalivatewhenthebellrings.HeiDIisasigni cantrevisionofthemodelofPavlovianconditioningdevelopedbyRescorlaandWagner(Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner&Rescorla,1972),andre ectsPavlov'svisionthatthestudyofconditioningprovidesassociativepsychologywithascienti cbasis(Pavlov,1941,171).Theirmodelhashadaprofoundandenduringin uenceonthe eldofanimallearning(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;McLaren,Kaye,&Mackintosh,1989;Pearce&Mackintosh,2010;Wagner,1981),butalsoonpsychologymorebroadly(e.g.,Gluck&Bower,1988;Kruschke,1992;Rumelhart,Hinton,&Williams,1986),andonneuroscience(e.g,Leeetal.,2018;Schultz,Dayan,&Montague,1997);with8667citationsatthetimeofwritingthisthesis.However,theRescorla-Wagnermodelo ersonlythemostrudimentaryanalysisoftheassociativestructuresthatareacquiredduringconditioningandhowthesemapontochangesinbehaviour.Moreover,themodelprovidesnoexplanationforrecentevidence,wheredi erentbehaviouralindicesoflearningcanbetakentosupportdi erentconclusionsaboutthestrengthofanassociation(e.g.,Flagel,Akil,&Robinson,59 2009;Flageletal.,2011;Patitucci,Nelson,Dwyer,&Honey,2016).Thisfundamentalproblem,togetherwithothersthatIshallcometo(e.g.,Dickinson,Hall,&Mackintosh,1976;Lubow,1989;Miller,Barnet,&Grahame,1995;Rescorla,2000,2001),providedtheimpetusforthedevelopmentofHeiDI.Thenameofthemodel,HeiDI,re ectstheliteraryreferenceandlinkstomysurnameandthoseofmysupervisorstooneoftheprincipalissuesthatthemodelseekstoaddress:Howexcitationandinhibitiondetermineideo-motion.3.1TheRescorla-WagnerModelTheRescorla-Wa

gnermodelproposesthatPavlovianconditionedbehaviourre ectstheformationofanassociationbetweenthenodesactivatedbytheconditionedstimulus(CS)andunconditionedstimulus(US).ThepresentationoftheCScomestoassociativelyactivatetherepresentationorideaoftheUSandtherebybehaviour,whichcanbethusconsideredideo-motive:Aseeminglyre exivemovemente ectedinresponsetoanidea,inthiscasetheevokedmemoryoftheUS.Themodelhasbeenfundamentaltothedevelopmentoftheoreticaltreatmentsofassociativelearningforalmost50yearsandhasin uencedneurobiologicalanalysesoflearningandmemory.Ibrie yreviewthemodelherebecauseitprovidestheprincipalsourceofinspirationforthenewmodelthatisdevelopedintheremainderofChapter3.AccordingtotheRescorla-Wagnermodel,thechangeintheassociativestrength(VCS�US)ofaCSonagiventrialisdeterminedbythedi erencebetweenthemaximumassociativestrengthsupportablebyaUS()andthepooledassociativestrengthofallstimulipresentedonthattrial(PVTotal�US).Theglobalorpoolederrorterm(-PVTotal�US)allowsthemodeltoaccommodateawiderangeofphenomena:blocking(e.g.,Kamin,1969),conditionedinhibition,(e.g.,Rescorla,1969),contingencye ects(e.g.,Rescorla,1968),overshadowing(e.g.,Mackintosh,1976),relativevalidity(e.g.,Wagner,Logan,Haberlandt,&Price,1968),superconditioning(e.g.,Rescorla,1971).Thesephenomenawerebeyondthescopeofmodelswithseparateerrortermsforeach60 componentofapatternofstimulation(e.g.,Bush&Mosteller,1951;Hull,1943).Italsoprovidesanelegantintegrationofexcitatoryconditioning,wherethememoryofaCSprovokesthememoryoftheUS,andinhibitorylearning,whereaCScanreducethelikelihoodoftheUSmemoryfrombecomingactivewhenitotherwisewould.VCS�

;US= CS�US(�XVTotal�US)(0)Brie y,thepoolederrortermmeansthatVCS�USisa ectednotonlybythecurrentassociativestrengthofthatstimulus(i.e.,VCS�US),butalsobythepresenceofotherstimulithathaveassociativestrength(i.e.,byPVTotal�US).AccordingtotheRescorla-Wagnermodel,thechangeinassociativestrengthdrivenbythediscrepancywithinthepoolederrorterm(-PVTotal�US)ismodulatedbytheproductoftwolearningrateparameters, CSand US.RescorlaandWagner(1972)notethat\thevalueof CSroughlyrepresentsstimulussalience"andthat\theassignmentofdi erent USvaluestodi erentUSsindicatesthattherateoflearningmaydependontheparticularUSemployed".Thetwolearningrateparameterswerecon nedtotheunitinterval:0 CS, US1,andenabledthemodeltocapturethefactthatthesalienceoftheCS( CS)andnatureoftheUS( US)a ecttherateofexcitatorylearning1.Ofparticularnote,however,isthefactthatthismodelofPavlovianconditioningdidnotaddress{inanysystematicfashion{thein uenceofassociativestrength(i.e.,V)onconditionedresponding.Indevelopingtheirmodelanditsapplicationtoexperimental ndings,RescorlaandWagner(1972,p.77)notedthatitwas\sucientsimplytoassumethatthemappingofVsintomagnitudeorprobabilityofconditionedrespondingpreservestheirordering",andthatanysuchmappingwouldinevitablydependonthedetailsofeachexperimen- 1ToenableinhibitoryconditioningtooccurontrialswhentheUSisabsent,RescorlaandWagner(1972)assumedthat takesapositivevaluewhentheUSisabsentbuttheCSispresent;withthisvalueassumedtobelowerthanontrialswhenboththeCSandUSarepresent.ThiscomplexityisavoidedinHeiDI.61 talsituationandon\perfo

rmance"factors.Inacompanionpaper,whencomparingconditioninginvolvingasingleCSwithconditioninginvolvingacompoundoftwoCSs,theyalsonoted\thatthegreaterthenumberofcueswhichismadeavailable,themorelikelyitisthatthesubjectwillbeprovided(andperhapsidiosyncraticallyso)withasinglesalientcuetowhichconditioningcanrapidlyoccur"(Wagner&Rescorla,1972,pp.303-304).Thisstatementacknowledges(parenthetically)thefactthatindividualdi erencesmighta ectconditioning(seealso,Pavlov,1941,pp.378-378)buttherehasbeenlittleappetitetoaddresssuchdi erences(empiricallyortheoretically)andtomovebeyondsimple(grouplevel)assumptionsaboutthetranslationoflearningintoperformance(seealso,Mackintosh,1975;Miller&Matzel,1988;Pearce,1994;Pearce&Hall,1980).However,thereisnowevidencedemonstratingthattherelianceonsuchassumptionscannolongerbesustained;andnorcantheideathatPavloviancondition-ingresultsinunconditionedresponsessnippedfromtheUSbeinggraftedontotheCSthroughaprocessofstimulussubstitution(seeDwyer,Burgess,&Honey,2012;Pavlov,1927;Wagner&Brandon,1989).3.2Individualdi erencesIthasbeendiscussedinChapter1,thatevenasimpleauto-shapingprocedurepro-ducesmarkedindividualdi erencesinbehaviour:someratspredominantlyinteractwiththeCS(e.g.,lever),othersinvestigatethelocationwherethereinforcerisabouttobedelivered,andtheremaindershowpatternsofbehaviourinbetweenthesetwoextremes(e.g.,Patituccietal.,2016).Whenleveractivityisusedastheassayofdiscriminationlearning,thesign-trackinggroupshowbetterlearningthanthegoal-trackinggroup;butwhenfood-wellactivityisusedthenthereverseisthecase.Thatis,itisnotpossibletoprovideamappingofVsontoconditionedbehaviourthatprovide

sacoherentinter-pretation:Focussingononemeasure(e.g.,sign-tracking)leadstotheconclusionthatassociativelearninghadproceededmorereadilyinonesetofratsthantheother,whilefocussingonthesecondmeasure(e.g.,goal-tracking)leadstotheoppositeconclusion.62 Asitstands,theRescorla-Wagnermodelisunabletoexplainwhy,foranygivenrat,oneresponsewasstrongerthantheother,andwhyinsomeratsgoal-trackingwasstrongerthansign-trackingwhereasinotherratsthisrelationshipwasreversed.Infact,theseresultsposeaproblemforanytheoryoflearningthatassumesamonotonicrelationshipbetweenasingleconstructthatrepresentslearningandacquiredbehaviour(e.g.,Gallistel&Gibbon,2000;Stout&Miller,2007).3.3HeiDI:Rationale,architectureandoverarchingassumptionsThepurposeofHeiDIistoo eranaccountinwhichtheassociativestructuresthatareacquiredduringPavlovianconditioningareintegratedwithananalysisofhowtheknowledgeembodiedinthesestructuresdeterminesthenatureoftheresponseselicitedbyaCS,andtheirrelativestrengths.Indoingso,themodelseekstoaddresschallengestotheRescorla-Wagnermodel,andothermodelsofPavlovianlearning(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Wagner,1981).Figure7providesaschematicfortheassociativestructures,towhichIwillaligntheanalysisofthelearningandperformanceequationsthatfollow.Theleft-handpanelshowsthestructureofthemodelbeforeconditioninghastakenplaceandtheright-handpanelshowsthestructureofthemodelafterconditioning.Beforeconditioning,theCSisprincipallylinkedtoasetofunconditionedresponses(r1-r3;e.g.,orienting,leverapproach,rearing),whereastheUSisweaklylinkedtoasetofunconditionedresponses(r4-r6;e.g.,food-wellapproach,chewing,swallowing).UnconditionedlinksfromtheCStor4-r6a

ndtheUStor1-r3areassumedtobeveryweak;andtheweightsofthelinesbetweentheCSandr1-r6andbetweenUSandr1-r6denotetherelativestrengthsoftheseuntrainedorunconditionedlinks.Inthisway,ageneraldistinctionisadoptedbetweenCS-orientedresponses(r1-r3)andUS-orientedresponses(r4-6;seeHolland,1977,1984).Importantly,asaconsequenceofconditioningitisassumedthatreciprocalCS-USand63 ]Figure7:Aschematicforassociativestructuresthatunderpinthetranslationofex-citatorylearningintoperformance.Theleft-handsidedepictsthemodelbeforecon-ditioning(i.e.,theunconditionedstructure),withthedarknessofthelinksindicatingtheirstrength,andtheright-handsidedepictsthemodelafterconditioning(i.e.,theconditionedstructure).ConditioningresultsinchangesinthestrengthofthereciprocalCS-USandUS-CSassociationsbetweennodesactivatedbytheCSandUS(denotedbythehashedlines).US-CSassociationsareacquired,whicharedepictedasthepresenceofhashedlinesintheconditionedstructure.Thegeneralrationaleforthisassumption,whichdoesnotfeatureinotherformalmodelsofPavlovianconditioning(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972),isoutlinednext.Amorespeci cjusti cationisreserveduntilthelearningrulesforthesereciprocalassociationsarepresented.IwillpresentinlatersectionshowtheinclusionofUS-CSassociations,aswellasCS-USassociations,providesthebasisforHeiDItoexplainawiderangeofphenomena:Inparticular,thosethathaveprovendiculttoreconcilewiththeRescorla-Wagnermodel,unequalchangeintheassociativestrengthsofthecomponentsofacompound(e.g.,Rescorla,2000),downshiftunblocking,(e.g.,Dickinson,Hall,&Mackintosh,1976)orthathavebeentakentoprovidesupportformodelsthathaveemphasized\predictiv

eness"(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980).64 TheformationofreciprocalassociationsbetweentheCSandUSnodescreatesafunctionalcellassemblyandenables\resonance"betweenthem:WhentheCSispre-sentedactivationpropagatestotheUSnode,whichispropagatedbacktotheCS(e.g.,Grossberg,1980).Arcediano,Escobar,andMiller(2005),inaseriesofexperimentswithbothhumanandnon-humansubjects,providedevidenceoftemporalintegrationbetweenthepre-sentationoftwodi erenttrainingexperiences(Experiment1:outcome!S1andS2!S1;Experiment2(A-C):S2!S1andUS!S1)basedontheelementcommontobothexperiences(S1).Thistemporalintegrationsuggeststheformationnotonlyofspeci ctemporalforwardassociations(i.e.,S2!S1)butalsoofspeci ctemporalbackwardassociations(i.e.,US S1).Thereisfurtherevidencethatsuchreciprocalassociationsareacquiredduringfor-wardconditioninginavarietyofpreparations(e.g.,Asch&Ebenholtz,1962;Cohen-Hatton,Haddon,George,&Honey,2013;Honey&Bolhuis,1997;Honey&Ward-Robinson,2002);andacomplementaryliteratureontheconditionsunderwhichUS-CSpairingsresultinconditionedrespondingtotheCS(e.g.,Barnet&Miller,1996;Cole&Miller,1999).Atatheoreticallevel,intypicalPavlovianconditioningprocedures{wheretheCSprecedesbutdoesnotco-existwiththeUS{thememorytraceoftheCSmustbesucienttosupportthedevelopmentofexcitatoryassociations(cf.Barnet&Miller,1996;Gallistel,1990;Miller,Barnet,&Grahame,1995;Wagner,1981).Im-portantly,whilethedevelopmentoftheCS-USassociationincreasesthelikelihoodthatthepresentationoftheCSwillactivatetheUSandtherebyprovoker4-r6,withoutthebackwardassociationstherewouldbelittlechangeinthelikelihoodthattheCSwouldprovoker1-r3.TheCS-USassociationallowsthepresen

tationoftheCStoactivatetheUSnodeandUS-CSassociationallowsactivationoftheUSnodetoincreaseactivationintheCSnode,whichincreasesthetendencyforr1-r3tobecomeactiveasaconsequenceofconditioning.WhenaCSispresented,therearetwosourcesofinformationthatareimmediately65 availabletoananimaluponwhichperformancecouldbebased:TheperceivedsalienceoftheCS,(whichisrelatedto CS)andtheperceivedsalienceoftheUSthatisactivatedbytheCS(whichisrelatedtoVCS�US).Afullye ectiveCSisheldtoactivatetheUSrepresentationtothevalueoftheperceivedsalienceofthepresentedUS(whichrelatesto US).HeiDIassumesthatbothofthesesourcescontributetothenatureofperformance(cf.Hull,1949).Inparticular,themodelproposesthattherelativevaluesof CSandVCS�USdeterminehowlearningistranslatedintoperformancethroughtwovalues,RCSandRUS.InadvanceofdescribingindetailhowRCSandRUSarecalculated,forthetimebeingsimplyassumethatif CS�VCS�US,thenRCS�RUS,whereasifVCS�US� CSthenRUS�RCS.ReturningtoFigure7,RCSa ectsbehaviourviaconnectionsfromtheCStor1-r6inFigure7,andRUSa ectsbehaviourviaconnectionsfromtheUStor1-r6.Themodelassumesthattheprecisenatureofthe(alternative)responsesgeneratedinagivenconditioningpreparationwillbeafunctionoftheinteractionbetweenthenatureoftheCSandUS(Holland,1977,1984).Inthenextsections,I rstpresentthelearningrulesusedbyHeiDItodeterminethedevelopmentofthereciprocalCS-USandUS-CSassociations(Equations1and2);andprovideasimpleruleforcombiningthesevaluesuponpresentationoftheCS(Equation3).Itisworthbrie ynotingthatEquations1and2embodytheideathattheperceivedsalienceoftheCSandUS,andtheirassociativelygenerated

counterparts,thatdetermineslearning.Thissuggestionisconsistentwiththeideathatindividualdi erencesintheperceivedsalienceoftheCSandUSplayacentralroleindeterminingindividualdi erencesintheexpressionoflearning.Ithenprovideadetailedanalysisofhowthiscombinedvalueisdistributedinperformance(Equations4-6).Thecorrespondingsimulationsoflearningandperformancearethenpresentedineachexperimentalchapter.Finally,IillustratehowHeiDIprovidesanaturalaccountforphenomenathatchallengetheRescorla-Wagnermodel,andhowitprovidesalternativeanalysesforresultsthathaveprovidedthebasisformodelsofPavlovianlearningthatincludelearntchangesinattentionorassociability(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980).66 3.4LearningrulesTheuseofapoolederrortermwasthecentralcontributionoftheRescorla-Wagnermodel,allowingittoprovideareadyaccountoftheconditionsunderwhichexcitatoryandinhibitorylearningoccur.VCS�US= CS(c US�XVTotal�US)(1)VUS�CS= US(c CS�XVTotal�CS)(2)HeiDIadoptsversionsofthepoolederrorterminEquation1andEquation2,fortheformationofCS-USandUS-CSassociations,respectively(alistoftheHeiDIequa-tionsisavailableattheendofthechapter).ThereisrecentevidencethatprovidesdirectsupportforthisfeatureofHeiDIinthecontextofCS-orientedbehaviourandUS-orientedbehaviour:AleverCSthatprovokessign-trackingcanblocktheacquisitionofgoal-trackingtoanauditoryCS,andanauditorystimulusthatprovokesgoal-trackingcanblockacquisitionofsign-trackingtoaleverCS(e.g.,Derman,Schneider,Juarez,&Delamater,2018).However,asIshallshow,whileEquations1and2haveformallyequivalentpoolederrorterms,theirfunctionalpropertiesdi erwhenastimuluscom-pou

nd(AB)ispairedwithaUS:Equation1functionsasapoolederrortermfortheA-USandB-US,whereasEquation2functionsasaseparateerrortermfortheUS-AandUS-Bassociations.Brie y,Iwilllatershowhowthisobservationenablestheuseofapoolederrortermtobereconciledwithresultsshowingthatcompound(AB)condi-tioningcanresultinunequalchangesinconditionedrespondingtoAandBdependingontheirpriortraininghistories(e.g.,Allman,Ward-Robinson,&Honey,2004;Rescorla,2000,2001).AnimportantfeatureofEquation1isthattheperceivedsalienceoftheUS( US)sets67 themaximumperceivedvalueoftheUSretrievedbytheCS(i.e.,VCS�US).Similarly,theperceivedsalienceoftheCSinEquation2( CS)setsthemaximumperceivedvalueoftheCSretrievedbytheUS(i.e.,VUS�CS).Theideathattheperceivedsalienceofthedirectlyactivatedandassociativelyactivatedrepresentationsofthestimuli(CSandUS)determineassociativechangereceivesdirectsupportfromresultsreportedbyDwyer,Figueroa,Gasalla,andLopez(2018).Theyexaminedthedevelopmentofa avourpreferencethroughpairinga avourCSwithan8%sucroseUS.Theyobservedthatprecedingthisconcentrationofsucrosebyeither2%sucrose(generatingpositivecontrast)or32%sucrose(generatingnegativecontrast)a ectedtheacquisitionofthe avourpreference:The avourpreferencesupportedby8%sucrosewaslargerwhenitwasprecededby2%sucrosethanwhenitwasprecededby32%sucrose.Moreover,whenthechangesintheperceivedsalienceoftheUS(8%sucrose)producedbycontrastweredirectlyassessed,throughtheanalysisoflickingmicrostructure,theydirectlycorrelatedwiththesizeoftheresultingpreferencefortheCS avours.3.4.1ExcitatorylearninganderrorcorrectionEquations1and2aresymmetricalrulesgoverningtheformationofCS-USandUS-CSassociat

ions,respectively.Equation1representsasimpli cationtotheRescorla-Wagnerlearningrule(Equation0)anddeterminestheformationofCS-USassociations;andEquation2providestheformallyequivalentruleforUS-CSassociations.WhileEquations1and2areformallyequivalentpoolederrorcorrectingrules,theyhavequitedi erentfunctionalpropertiesinconventionalconditioningproceduresinwhichacom-poundoftwoCSs(AB)precedesaUS.Inshort,Equation1functionsasapoolederrorterminconventionalcompoundconditioningprocedures(Rescorla&Wagner,1972)whereasEquation2functionsasaseparateerrorterminsuchprocedures(Bush&Mosteller,1951;Hull,1943).However,itisalsoworthnotingthatthemodelpredictsthatifasingleCSweretobefollowedbyacompoundoftwoUSs(US1andUS2),thentheassociationofUS1withtheCSwouldbeweakerthanifUS1hadbeenpairedwith68 theCSinisolation.ThepredictionthattherewillbecompetitionorovershadowingbetweenthecapacityoftwoUSstobecomeassociatedwithasingleCShasreceivedempiricalsupport(seeMiller&Matute,1998).InEquation1, CSisalearningrateparametercon nedtotheunitinterval0 CS1,andc* USdeterminestheasymptotefortheCS-USassociation;whereasinEquation2, USisalearningrateparametercon nedtotheunitinterval0 US1,andc* CSdeterminestheasymptotefortheUS-CSassociation.Notethat CSand USaredimensionlessscalars,butwhentheyserveastheasymptotesforassociativestrengththeyaremultipliedbyaconstantof1inunitsofV(c).TherequirementforcisthathasunitsofV,butthenumericvalueisnot xedbythisrequirement.Theconstantcisassumedheretobe1forsimplicity.However,theasymptoticlimitsoflearningneednotbe USinEquation1or CSinEquation2,butsomemultip

leofthesevalues.WhentheCSisabsent, CSandc* CSaresetto0andwhentheUSisabsent USandc* USaresetto0.InkeepingwiththeRescorla-Wagnermodel, CSand USareassumedtore ecttheperceivedsalienceoftheCSandUS,respectively.AccordingtoEquation1,thestrengthoftheassociationfromtheCStotheUS(i.e.,VCS�US)convergesasymptoticallyonc* US.ThechangeinthestrengthoftheassociationbetweenCSandtheUSonagiventrial(VCS�US)isdeterminedbytheerrorordi erencewithinthepoolederrorterm(c* US{PVTotal�US);andPVTotal�USdenotesthenetassociativestrengthofallofthestimulipresentedonthattrial.DuringsimpleCS-USpairings,excitatorylearningceaseswhenPVTotal�US=c* US;andthelearningrateparameter CSa ectstherateatwhichVCSapproachesc* US.Inthiscase,thepoolederrortermmeansthattheacquisitionofassociativestrengthbyagivenstimuluswillbein uencedbytheassociativestrengthofotherstimulithataccompanyit;forexample,whenacompoundoftwostimuli(AandB)ispairedwithaUS.Equation2isthecomplementarylearningrulegoverningtheformationoftheUS-CSassociation.ThechangeinthestrengthofthisassociationVUS�CSonagiventrialisalsodeterminedbythediscrepancywithinthepoolederrorterm(c* CS{PVTotal�CS).69 CSre ectsthesalienceoftheCS,andPVTotal�CSdenotestheassociativestrengthoftheUS(intypicalconditioningprocedures).LearningceaseswhenPVTotal�CS=c* CS,andthelearningrateparameter USa ectstherateatwhichVUS�CSapproachesc* CS.BecauseinatypicalPavlovianconditioningprocedurethereisonlyoneUS(cf.Miller&Matute,1998),thec* CSvalueofeachCSinacompound(e.g.,AandB)setstheasymptotefortheassociationfromtheUStothatC

S.ThismeansthattheUS-CSassociationswillproceedindependentlyforeachofthecomponentsofacompoundthatispairedwithaUS.Thatis,whileEquation1hasboththeformalandfunctionalpropertiesofEquation(0)andpredictsthesamephenomenaasthatmodel,Equation2hasequivalentformalproperties,butfunctionsinthesamewayasaseparateerrortermduringcompoundconditioning(e.g.,Bush&Mosteller,1951;Hull,1943).HowcouldonetestwhethertheanalysisprovidedbyEquations1and2isaccurate?Consider rstthesimplecaseinwhichtwoCSs(AandB)arepresentedtogetherandpairedwithaUS.Undertheseconditions,theassociativestrengthaccruedbyA(VA�US)andB(VB�US)willbelessthanifthesestimulihadbeenseparatelypairedwiththeUS.Ane ectknownasovershadowing(e.g.,Mackintosh,1976).However,thestateofa airswillbedi erentforthereciprocalassociations(i.e.,VUS�AandVUS�B).Theywillundergothesamechangeinassociativestrengthastheywouldhavedonehadconditioningwitheachoccurredinisolation;becausethec* Aandc* BforstimulusAandBsetseparateasymptotesfortheassociationbetweentheUS-AandUS-B.Ofcourse,the ndingthatovershadowingisobservedundersuchconditionsisuninformative;becauseEquations0and1predictionthatVA�USwillbelowerwhenithasbeenconditionedincompoundwithBthanwhenithasbeenconditionedalone.But,nowimaginethesamecompoundconditioningscenario,butthatonthisoccasionapreviousstageoftraininghadestablishedAasconditionedexciter(bypairingitwithaUS)andBhadbeenestablishedasaconditionedinhibitor(bypairingitpairedwiththeabsenceofanotherwisepredictedUS).AccordingtoEquations0and1,providedAandBareequallyintense(i.e., A= B)thentheyshouldgainequivalentassociative70 strengthasaconsequenceoft

heABcompoundbeingpairedwiththeUS.However,accordingtoEquation2,whiletheassociationbetweentheUSandAwillnotincrease(havingreachedasymptoteduringthe rststage)theassociationbetweentheUSandBwillincrease,becausetheUShadnotpreviouslybeenpairedwithB.Ifthechangesinthereciprocalassociationsweretobecombined,thenBshouldhavegainedgreatercombinedassociativestrengththanA.Rescorla(2000,2001)haspublishedaseriesofingeniousexperimentsthathascon rmedthispredictionunderavarietyofcircumstances(seealsoAllman&Honey,2005;Allman,Ward-Robinson,&Honey,2004;Holmes,Chan,&Westbrook,2019).Iwillprovideaformalsimulationoftheanalysisoftheseresults,whichhavebeentakentoimplicateseparateerrortermsinPavlovianconditioning,oncetherulesforcombiningthereciprocalassociationshasbeendescribed,andthewayinwhichassociativestrengtha ectsperformancepresented.Asjustnoted,CS-USpairingscreateafunctionalcellassemblythroughreciprocalassociationsbetweentheCSandUS.Tocapturethisinteractionandtosimplifytheperformancerules,itisdesirabletocombinethenetassociativestrengthsoftheCS-USassociationreturnedbyEquation1(forVCS�US)andtheUS-CSassociationreturnedbyEquation2(forVUS�CS).Thecombinedassociativestrengthwithinthisassembly(VCOMB)isgivenbyEquation3a2.Thecombinedassociativestrengthofacompoundstimulus(VCOMB�AB)composedoftwoCSs(AandB)isgivenbyEquation3b;inwhichPVAB�USisthesumofVA�USandVB�US,andVUS�AandVUS�BarethestrengthsoftheassociationsbetweentheUSandA,andtheUSandB.VCOMB=VCS�US+(1 cVCS�USVUS�CS)(3a)VCOMB�AB=XVCS�US+[1 cXVAB�US(VUS�A+VUS�B)](3b) 2ThesymmetricalcombinationrulescanbeusediftheUS(ratherthanth

eCS)wastestedalone,e.g.,VUS�CS+(1/c*VUS�CS*VCS�US)71 ThischoiceofcombinationrulerecognizesthefactthatwhiletheCSdirectlyacti-vatestheCS-USassociation,theUS-CSassociationisonlyindirectlyactivatedbythepresentationoftheCS.Therulehasthegeneralpropertythatthedirectlyactivatedlinkinachainofassociationswillconstraintheimpactoftheindirectlyactivatedlinkonperformance.ThisistrueregardlesswhetherthestimulusisaCSoraUS.IftheCSactivatestherepresentationoftheUS,VCS�USwillconstraintheimpactonperfor-manceofVUS�CS,howeveriftheUSactivatestherepresentationoftheCS,VUS�CSwillconstraintheimpactonperformanceofVCS�US.Forsimplicity,whenpresentingaCS,ifVCS�USwas0andVUS�CSwaspositive,thenVCOMB(andhencebothRCSandRUS)0inspiteofthefactthattherelationshipbetweentheCSandUShadbeenencoded(i.e.,asVCS�US).Thesigni canceofthispropertyinthecontextofHeiDIwillbecomeapparentwhentheblockingphenomenonisconsideredingreaterdetail.(e.g.,Kamin,1969).3.4.2ExtinctionWhenconditioningtrialswithaCSarefollowedbyextinctiontrialswheretheCSispresented,butnoUSoccurs,c* USissetto0andPVTotal�USwillbepositive.Undertheseconditions,Equation1returnsanegativevalueforVCS�US,butEquation2returns0forVUS�CS(because US=0).Itisworthhighlightingthisasymmetrybetweenwhatislearnedduringconditioningandextinction:excitatorylearninginvolveschangestoVCS�USandVUS�CS,butconventionalextinctionproceduresinvolveonlychangestoVCS�US.ThenegativevaluesreturnedbyEquation1duringextinctioncanbeinterpretedintwoways:First,theycoulddenotethegrowthofnegativeassociativestrength(Konorski,1948;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner&Rescorla,1972

).Second,theycoulddenotetheformationofanexcitatoryassociationbetweentheCSanda`NoUS'node,whichinturninhibitstheUSnodeandtherebyreducesconditionedbehaviour(seeKonorski,1967;Pearce&Hall,1980;Zimmer-Hart&Rescorla,1974).In72 the rstcase,thenegativevaluesaredirectlyre ectedintheunderpinningassociativestructure,andinthesecondcasetheyre ecttheproductofanexcitatoryCS-NoUSassociationmultipliedbyaninhibitoryNoUS-USassociation.However,accordingtobothinterpretations,thenetassociativestrengthoftheforwardassociationinvolvingtheCS(VCS�US)isthesumofthepositiveandnegativeassociativevaluesreturnedbyEquation1;andthenetassociativestrengthofVCS�USisthesumofthepositiveandnegativevaluesreturnedbyEquation2.NegativevaluesofVUS�CSwillbereturnedbyEquation2whenPVTotal�CS�c* CS.ThissituationwouldarisewhentheUSispresentedaloneafterconditioninghastakenplaceorifadditionalUSswerepresentedintheinter-trialintervalsbetweenCS-USpairings.BothofthesemanipulationsresultinareductioninconditionedrespondingtotheCS(seeRescorla,1968,1973)3.Infact,accordingtoHeiDIwhilebothofthesemanipulationswillresultinextinctionoftheUS-CSassociation,addingUSpresentationsduringtheintervalsbetweenCS-UStrialsallowtheformationofacontext-USassociation,whichshouldblockthedevelopmentoftheCS-USassociation.Inkeepingwiththisanalysis,ithasbeenarguedthatthee ectsofmanipulatingCS-UScontingency,byaddingUSalonepresentationsduringconditioning,mightbemultiplydetermined(Baker&Mercier,1982).Latersimulationswillcon rmthedescriptionoftheconsequencesofextinctionpre-sentedinthepreviousparagraph.Forthetimebeing,itisimportanttonotethataccordingtoEquations1and2,ext

inctionleavesasigni cantcontributiontoperfor-mancecompletelyunchanged(i.e.,theUS-CSassociation,VUS�CS),ratherthansimplybeingcounteractedbyadditionalinhibitorylearning,asisthecasewiththenetCS-USassociation(VCS�US).ThisfeatureofHeiDIisconsistentwiththegeneralobservationthatpost-extinctionmanipulationscanrevealthepresenceofresidualexcitationinper-formance,whichhasrepresentedanongoingchallengetotheRescorla-Wagnermodel(e.g.,Bouton,2004).ManipulationsthatenabletheUStobeactivated(orthatdisrupt 3ThereisalsoevidencethatwhenCS-USpairingsarefollowedbyseparatepresentationsofthesameUSbutatahighersalience(calledUSin ation)theCRtotheCSisampli ed(Bouton,1984;Rescorla,1973).Undertheseconditions,inadditiontoanyreductioninnetVUS�CS,presentationsofahighersalienceUSmightchangetheresponseunitsactivatedbytheUS,whichcoulda ectlaterperformancetotheCS.73 theCS-NoUSassociation)willresultinareturninperformancetotheCS.3.4.3InhibitorylearningIfconditioningtrialsinwhichstimulusAispairedwithaUSareintermixedwithtrialsonwhichAispresentedwithstimulusBandtheUSisnotdelivered,thennon-reinforcedABtrialswillresultinareductioninthenetassociativestrengthofA,andBwillbecomeanetinhibitor.ThenetassociativestrengthofABisgivenbyaddingthepositiveandnegativevaluesreturnedbyEquation1forstimulusAandB.ThenetassociativestrengthoftheUS,VUS�CS,isthesumofthepositiveandnegativeassociativevaluesreturnedbyEquation2.AccordingtoEquation2,onnon-reinforcedABtrialstherewillbenochangeintheUS-AorUS-Bassociations;againbecause US=0.However,inhibitorylearningcanalsobeproducedifABispairedwithaUSthatissmallerinmagnitudethantheUSthatispairedwithA(e.g.,Cotto

n,Goodall,&Mackintosh,1982).Undertheseconditions, US�0andHeiDIpredictsthattherewouldbeanincreaseintheexcitatorystrengthoftheUS-AandUS-Bassociations,whichwouldcontributetothevaluesofVCOMBforA,BandAB.Thepredictionthatconventionalconditionedinhibitiontrainingandconditionedinhibitionproducedbyareductioninreinforcermagnituderesultindi erentassociationstructureshasnotbeenevaluated.3.5PerformancerulesWhenaCSispresentedtherearetwosourcesofinformationthatareavailabletoananimal,theperceivedsalienceoftheCS(relatedto CS)andtheassociativestrengthofthatCS(VCS�US),whichcanbeconsideredanestimateof USgivenitsrelationshipwithrelationshipwithc* US.Thesetwosourcesofinformationareheldtodeterminethenatureofconditionedbehaviour.74 RCS=c CS c CS+jVCS�USjVCOMB(4)RUS=jVCS�USj c CS+jVCS�USjVCOMB(5)Equation4andEquation5generatetwovalues,RCS,andRUS,whichre ecttheproportionsof CSandVCS�US.Asbefore,intheseequations, CSismultipliedbycinordertotranslateitintounitsofV.Todeterminetheextenttowhichtheseproportionsa ecttheabsolutelevelsofperformancetheyaremultipliedbytheoverallassociativepropertiesoftheCS-USensemble(i.e.,VCOMB).RCSa ectsbehaviourviaconnectionsfromtheCStor1-r6,andRUSa ectsbehaviourviaconnectionsfromtheUStor1-r6(seeFigure7).BecauseinthesimulationspresentedherenetVCS�US�0,therealvaluesofVCS�UScanbeusedtodetermineRCSandRUSinEquations4and5.However,toaddressthefactthatEquation1(andEquation2)canreturnnegativevalues,theuseofabsolutevaluesensuresthattheproportionsinEquations4and5are1.Thischoicealsoleavesopenthepossibilitythatanetinhibitorco

uldprovokerespondingwhenpresentedalone(cf.Konorski,1967;Pearce&Hall,1980),ratherthanhavingnoe ectonperformanceunlessitispresentedwithanexcitor(Konorski,1948;Wagner&Rescorla,1972).Fornow,itissucienttonotethatEquation4returnsahighervalueforRCSasthevalueofc* CSincreasesrelativetothevalueofVCS�US,andEquation5returnsahighervalueforRUSasVCS�USincreasesrelativetoc* CS.Thesetwoequationsarereadilyextendedtoaccommodatestimuluscompounds(AB).Todoso,thec* valuesforAandBaresimplycombined(e.g.,added)toformc* AB,andthenetVsofAandBarecombined(e.g.,added)toformVAB�US.Similarly,agivenstimulus(CSorUS)canbeconceivedofasasetofelementswiththeirownc* valuesandnetVs,whichcouldbeenteredintoEquations4and5usingthesameapproach(cf.Atkinson&Estes,1963;Delamater,2012;Wagner&Brandon,1989).WhileEquations4and5provideasimplebasisforthedistributionoftheassociative75 propertiesoftheCS-USensemble(i.e.,VCOMB)totheresponse-generatingunits(r1-r6)thoughRCSandRUS,theydonotspecifyhowtheseresponseunitsonceactivateda ectbehaviour.OnesimplepossibilityisthatagivenvalueofRCS,forexample,resultsinthesameamountofCS-orientedresponding(r1-r3)irrespectiveofthevalueofRUS.Thispossibilityequatestotherebeingparallelactivationoftheresponse-generatingunits(r1-r6),andisformallyexpressedinEquation6,whereRCSandRUSaretranslatedintodimensionlessvaluesbybeingmultipliedbythereciprocaloftheconstant,\c".AccordingtoEquation6,theactivationofagivenresponseunit(e.g.,r1)issimplydeterminedbyaddingtheproductsof(i)multiplyingRCSbytheconnectionbetweentheCSandr1(CS-r1),and(ii)multiplyingRUSbythestrengthofconnectionbetweentheUSandthesameresponseunit(e.

g.,US-r1).Therearemorecomplexpossibilitiesinvolvingtheinteractionbetweentheactivationvaluesoftheresponse-generatingunits(e.g.,McClelland&Rumelhart,1981),butfornowthisservesassimpleplaceholderforfuturetheoreticalelaboration.r1=(1 cRCSCSr1)+(1 cRUSUSr1)(6)ThesimulationspresentedinlatersectionsarederivedfromEquations1-5.Equation6simplyinvolvesmultiplyingtheresultingRCSandRUSvaluesbythe xedstrengthlinks(withvaluesbetween0and1)betweentheCSandUSnodesandtheresponseunits(e.g.,theCS-r1andUS-r1links).Ifthetwosetsoflinksareequivalent(seeFigure7),thendi erencesinactivationoftheresponseunitswilldependsolelyonRCSandRUS.3.5.1Individualdi erencesin USHeiDIassumesthat CSand USare xedforagivenCSandUSinagivenanimal,butproposethattheperceivedsalienceoftheCS(relatingto CS)andUS(relating76 to US),andhencec* CSandVCS�USinEquations4and5,canvarybetweenani-mals.Thisassumptionprovidesthebasisforindividualdi erencesinRCSandRUS,becausec* CSandVCS�USa ectperformanceaccordingtoEquations4and5(remem-berVCS�USconvergesonc* USatasymptote)4.Thisanalysisreceivessupportfromtheobservationthatrodentswhoshowedastronglikingforsucrose(asmeasuredbylickingmicrostructure;seeDwyer,2012)aremorelikelytobegoal-trackers(whensucrosewastheUS)thanthosewhoexhibitedaweakerlikingforsucrose(Patituccietal.,2016).Individualvariationinthepalatabilityofsucrosecanbereadilyalignedtodi erencesin USthatwilla ectbothlearning(i.e.,theasymptoticvalueofVCS�US)andtherateatwhichVUS�CSreachesasymptote(throughEquations1and2)andthedistribu-tionofVCOMBinperformance(throughVCS�USinEquatio

ns3-6).Moreover,Dwyeretal.(2018)showedthatindividualdi erencesinthepalatabilityofsucrose(duringtheirexperimentsinvolvingcontraste ects)werepositivelycorrelatedwiththe avourpreferencelearning.Thereisadditionalevidencethatisconsistentwiththepropositionthat USfordi erentUSsvariesbetweenanimals,andindeedwithinagivenanimal:WhenseparatepresentationsoftwoleversarepairedwiththesameUS(e.g.,foodorsucrose)thenthebiastowardssign-trackingorgoal-trackingononelevercorrelateswiththebiasontheother,resultspresentedinExperiment2.However,whenthepresentationofoneleverispairedwithsucroseandtheotherleverispairedwithfood,thenthereisnocorrelationbetweenthebiasonthetwolevers(Patituccietal.,2016).Thispatternofresultsisconsistentwiththeviewthatthe USvaluesfortwoUSs(i.e.,foodandsucrose)canvarybetweenanimalsandwithinagivenanimal.Furtherevidence.AcentralpropositionofHeiDIisthatvariationinVCS�USinteractswithc* CStodetermineperformance.ThispropositionreceivessupportfromtheresultspresentedinExperiment1,inwhichaCSis rstpairedwithaUSisthenpresentedaloneacrossaseriesoftrials.Extinctiontrialsshoulda ectnetVCS�US,conditionalon 4Equations4and5canbetransformedforthecaseinwhichtheUSispresentedalone:Undertheseconditions,c* USreplacesc* AandVUS�CSreplacesVCS�US77 thereductionofc* USfromapositivevalueto0inEquation1,butnot CS.TheclearpredictionisthatwhilebothRCSandRUSshoulddecreaseduringextinction(VCOMBwillre ectthereductioninVCS�US;seeEquation3),Equations4and5predictthatthisdecreasewillbelessmarkedforRCSthanforRUS: CSwillremainthesameandVCS�USwillbelower.Thispredictionwascon rmedinrat

sthatweredesignatedaseithersign-trackersorgoal-trackersinExperiment1.Inbothgroups,thetendencyforratstointeractwiththelever(sign-tracking)extinguishedlessrapidlythaninteractingwiththefood-well(goal-tracking).Theresultsfromarelatedconditioningpreparationprovideconvergingevidencefromtheproposedinteractionbetweenc* CSandVCS�USindeterminingRCSandRUS.KayeandPearce(1984)gaveratspresentationsofalocalizedlightthatwaseitherpairedwiththedeliveryofafoodpelletoneverytrial(ingroupcontinuous)oronarandomlyscheduled50%ofoccasionsonwhichitispresented(ingrouppartial).Theyobservedthatwhenthelightwascontinuouslyreinforceditmaintainedahigherlevelofgoal-tracking(food-wellentries)andalowerlevelofsign-tracking(orientingandapproachtothelight)thanwhenthelightwaspartiallyreinforced(seealso,Anselme,Robinson,&Berridge,2013).AccordingtoEquation1and2,netVCS�USwillbehigherduringacontinuousthanapartialreinforcementschedule,andacontinuousreinforcementshouldresultinagreaterbiastowardsgoal-tracking(RUS)andasmallerbiastowardssign-tracking(RCS)thanpartialreinforcement,whichcouldresultintheoppositebias(seeEquations4and5).However,KayeandPearce(1984)alsoobservedthatsign-trackingwashigherinabsolutetermsduringpartialthancontinuousreinforcement.This ndingmightre ectthefactthathighlevelsofgoal-tracking,duringcontinuousreinforcement,weremorelikelytointerfere(atthelevelofresponseoutput)withsign-trackingthanthelowerlevelsofgoal-trackingengenderedbypartialreinforcement(seediscussionofEquation6).Inanycase,thefactthatCS-orientedbehaviourismaintainedbypartialreinforcementshouldalsoimproveananimal'slaterabilitytodetectnewrelationshipsinvolvingthatCS(cf

.Pearce&Hall,1980;Wilson,Boumphrey,&Pearce,1992).78 3.6SimulationsoflearningandperformanceSimulationsweredoneinaShinydashboardapp(HeiDI)usingRprogram-minglanguage.ThecodeisavailableontheOpenScienceFramework(OSF;https://osf.io/j8tps/?view only=c7e9286d80464cf1a244c9cface69b4e).Thesimulation gurespresentedinthethesisarebasedonthemodelfunctionscreatedintheapp(`model.R'),howeverthegraphicalrepresentationhasbeenadaptedtoincludeseveralsimulationsinthesame gure.ThecodeforthesimulationspresentedinthethesiscanbefoundontheOSFwebsite(https://osf.io/h4fyg/),morespeci callyat`The-sis/Simulations'.ThesoftwareusedfortheappwasR(version3.3.3)andRStudio(Version1.1.463.).ToreproducethesimulationsdownloadandinstallRsoftwarefortheappropriateoper-atingsystem.AftersuccessfullyinstallingR,downloadandinstallRStudio.Next,open`install packages.R' leandrunthe lewhichinstallsalltheneededpackages.Theonlineliveversionoftheappincludesa`Readme'(READMEfolder) lewithdetailedinstructionsforreproducingthecode.Thisdocumenthasatimestamp(everytimeitiscompiledthedateisupdated)andanychangesfromapreviousversionaredetailedin`log'folder.Eachupdateoftheapphasandwillhaveanumberedlog le.IntheAppendix(HeiDIapp)Iincludeamoredetailedexplanationoftheappstructureand les.3.6.1ExcitatoryconditioningandextinctionFigure8showssimulationsofacquisitionofconditioning(leftsideofeachpanel,Trials1-6)andextinction(right-sideofeachpanel,Trials7-12).PanelsAandCde-pictsimulationsofthedevelopmentoftheCS-USassociationderivedfromEquation1(VCS-US),theUS-CSassociationderivedfromEquation2(VUS-CS)andtheircom-binedvalues(VCOMB).MaximumVCS-USisdeterminedbyc

* USandmaximumVUS-CSisdeterminedbyc* CS.ThelearningrateatwhichVCS-USreachestheasymptoteis79 determinedbyc* CS(Equation1).ThelearningrateatwhichVUS-CSreachestheasymptoteisdeterminedbyc* US(Equation2).PanelsBandDshowhowEquations4and5translatelearningintotheperformancevaluesofRCS(CS-orientedresponses)andRUS(US-orientedresponses)acrossaseriesofCS-USpairings.SimulationswereconductedinwhicheitherRCSdominatedperformanceduringcon-ditioning(PanelAandB; CS=.50and US=.30;thisbiaswouldbeconsistentwithasign-tracker)orRUSdominatedperformance(panelsCandD; CS=.30and US=.50;thisbiaswouldbeconsistentwithagoal-tracker).When CS� US,RCSdominatedRUS,butwhen US� CSthenthereverseisthecase.Thegeneralconclusionisthatifc* CS=VCS-US,thenEquations4and5returnthesameorsimilarcontributionsforRCSandRUS;butifc* CS6=VCS-USthenthecomponentwiththelargestvalue(c* CSorVCS-US)contributesproportionatelymoretoperformance.Inallofthesimulationsthatfollow,itisassumedthattheconstant(c)is1inunitsofV.Therefore,thevaluesof CSand USarethesameasthoseofc* CSandc* US,respectively.Foreaseofpresentation,Iwillonlyreferto CSand USwithoutexplicitreferencetothetransformationsthatappearinEquations1-6.StartingwithpanelsAandB,when CSand UShavebeenarrangedforRCStodominateperformance,itisclearfrompanelAthatduringconditioningVUS�CS�VCS�US(when CS=.50and US=.30),andthatVCOMBissimilartoVUS�CS.Duringextinction,inspectionofpanelAshowsthatVCS�USandVCOMBdecline,butVUS�CSdoesnot(because US=0).PanelBshowsthatthereductioninRCSis(numerically)lessmar

kedthanRUS.MovingtopanelsCandD,when CSand UShavebeenarrangedforRUStodominateperformance,duringconditioningVCS�US�VUS�CSandVCOMB�VCS�US.Duringextinction,VCS�USandVCOMBdecline,butVUS�CSdoesnot.PanelDshowsthatthereductioninRCSoccursmuchlessrapidlythanthereductioninRUS.Forbothtypesofsimulatedrats,STs(PanelAandB)andGTs(PanelCandD),HeiDIpredictsthatUS-orientedbehaviour(goal-tracking)declinemorerapidlythanCSorientedbehaviour(sign-tracking).80 InExperiment1,ratsreceivedtraininginwhichonelever,(L1)wasfollowedbyareinforcerandanotherlever(L2)wasnon-reinforced.Duringthereversalstagethecontingenciesontheleverwerereversed:L1wasnon-reinforcedandL2wasnowrein-forced.TheresultsofExperiment1con rmHeiDI'sprediction,goal-trackingchangedmorerapidlythansign-trackingwhenthecontingencieswerereversed.Notethatinthesimulations,thee ectsofreversingcontingenciesatthebeginningoftrial6,inthesim-ulationsareevidentintrial7.Thisisbecausetrial7showsthechangesinassociativestrengthderivedfromeventsduringtrial6. Figure8:Simulationsofthee ectsofanextinctionprocedureonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking:Simulationforconditioning(trials1-6)andextinction(trials7-12)forthereinforced(+)lever.PanelsAandCdepicttheoutputvaluesforVCS-US,VUS-CS,VCOMB,andpanelsBandDshowthecorrespondingoutputvaluesforRCSandRUS.TheparametersduringconditioningwerechosentoresultinabiastowardsRCS(i.e., CS=.50and US=.30;panelsAandB)orabiastowardsRUS(i.e., CS=.30and US=.50;panelsCandD).Duringextinction, USwassetto0.81 3.6.2ChangeinUSvalueInExperiment2ratsreceivedduringtrainingseparatepresentationsoftwolevers(L1andL2),thatwer

ebothpairedwiththesamereinforcerduringtraining(eitherfoodpelletsorsucrose).Workinourlaboratoryindicatedthatfoodpelletsmaintainhigherlevelsofbothleverpressingandfood-wellentriesthandoessucrose;whichshouldbeevidentinthe rststageoftraining.Duringthesecondstage,thereinforcersassociatedwiththetwoleverswereswitched:theratsgivenpelletsduringtrainingreceivedsucroseduringtheswitchandthosegivensucroseduringtrainingreceivedfoodpelletsduringtheswitch.Becausethetransitionbetweenreinforcersdoesnota ectthestimulus( CS)inanyobviousway,changesinperformancemustre ecttheincreaseordecreasein USa ectsperformance.Thechangefromsucrose(low US)tofoodpellets(high US)increase US,whereasthechangefromfoodpellets(high US)tosucrose(low US)decrease US.ResultsfromExperiment2con rmthispredictionasithasbeenseenthatgoal-trackingwasmorea ectedbyachangeinUSvaluethansign-tracking.Figure9showssimulationsofacquisitionofconditioning(leftsideofeachpanel,Trials1-6)andswitchinUSvalue(right-sideofeachpanel,Trials7-12).PanelsAandCdepicttheoutputvaluesforVCS-US,VUS-CS,VCOMB,andpanelsBandDshowthecorrespondingoutputvaluesforRCSandRUS.TheparameterswerechosentoresultineitheranincreaseinUSvalueaftertheswitch( CS=.50, US(before)=.30and US(after)=.50;PanelAandB)oradecreaseinUSvalue( CS=.50, US(before)=.50and US(after)=.30;PanelCandD).ForHeiDImanipulationsthatchange USshoulda ectgoal-trackingand,asasecondaryconsequence,sign-tracking.InspectionofpanelsBandDshowsthatRUSismorea ectedbythechangeinUSvaluethanRCS.ThesesimulationsareimplementedinanopensourceappHeiDIapp.Moredetailsabouthow

IconductedthesimulationscanbefoundintheAppendix,sectionSimulations.InChapter4IpresentanddiscusssimulationsderivedfromHeiDIforinhibitoryconditioningandindividualdi erencesinUSvalue.Fornow,IdiscusshowusingapoolederrorterminEquations1and2enablesHeiDItoexplaine ectsthathaveposed82 achallengeforRescorla-Wagnermodel. Figure9:Simulationsofthee ectsofachangeinUSvalueonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking:Simulationforconditioning(trials1-6)andaftertheswitchinUSvalue(trials7-12).PanelsAandCdepicttheoutputvaluesforVCS-US,VUS-CS,VCOMB,andpanelsBandDshowthecorrespondingoutputvaluesforRCSandRUS.TheparameterswerechosentoresultineitheranincreaseinUSvalueaftertheswitch( CS=.50, US(before)=.30and US(after)=.50;PanelAandB)oradecreaseinUSvalue( CS=.50, US(before)=.50and US(after)=.30;PanelCandD).3.6.3CompoundconditioningandthepoolederrortermHeiDIprovidesapotentialreconciliationoftheuseofapoolederrortermswiththeobservationthatstimuliwithdi erentassociativehistoriesappeartoundergounequalchangewhentheyareconditionedincompound;anobservationthatwastakentobeinconsistentwiththeRescorla-Wagnermodelanditssuccessors(seeHolmesetal.,2019).Torecap:Inonesetofexperiments,Rescorla(2000)initiallytrainedratswithtwoexcitatorystimuli(AandC),eachfollowedbyreinforcement(A+,C+)aswellastwoinhibitorystimuli(BandD)eachnon-reinforcedinthepresenceofX(X+;XB-;XD-).Thenacompoundconsistingofoneexcitorandoneinhibitorisreinforced(AB+).83 Testingisconductedwithtwocompoundseachcontainingoneexcitorandoneinhibitor.Thetestcompoundsdi erinwhethertheycontaintheexcitor(AD)orinhibitor(BC)fromtheABreinforcementphase.LetassumethatAandCboth

hadexcitatoryassociativestrengthof.50,andBandDbothhadinhibitoryassociativestrengthof-.50beforethecompound,AB,waspairedwiththeUS(i.e.,AB-�US).AccordingtoEquations0and1,theassociativestrengthofbothshouldincreaseanequivalentamount:Afrom.50to.75andBfrom-.50to-.25.ThiswouldmeanthattheADcompoundshouldhaveanassociativestrengthof.25(.75+-.50)andtheBCcompoundshouldalsohaveanassociativestrengthof.25(.50+-.25).However,accordingtoHeiDIonealsoneedstoconsiderthefateofthebackwardassociationsduringcompoundconditioning:betweentheUSandA,andbetweentheUSandB.Ifitisassumedthat forallstimuliis.30,thenVUS�Awillbe.30bytheendofthe rststageoftraining,butVUS�Bwillbe0,becauseBhasnotbeenpairedwiththeUS.ThiswillmeanthatwhileVUS�AwillnotchangeduringpairingsofABwiththeUS(theasymptoteforVUS�Adeterminedby =.30willhavebeenreachedasaresultofthe rststageoftraining),VUS�Bcanincrease(e.g.,from0to.30).Thiswillmeanthatduringthetest,VCOMB�BCwillbehigherthanVCOMB�AD.Thisanalysisretainsapoolederrortermforallassociations,butrecognizesthefact{hithertounacknowledged{thatassociationsfromtheUStoAandBwillproceedindependentlyofoneanotherinconventionalconditioningprocedures(i.e.,whenthereisonlyasingleUS).Simulationscon rmtheaccuracyofthisanalysisacrossabroadrangeofparameters5,butintheinterestsofconsistencytheparametersweresetinthewaydescribedinthepreviousparagraph:The valuesofA,B,CandDweresetat.30;bytheendofstage1,VA�USandVC�USwere.50(i.e., US=.50)whileVB�USandVD�USwere{.50;andVUS�AandVUS�Cwere.30,whereasVUS�BandVUS�Dwere0.Havingsettheseparameters,IthensimulatedhowtheCS-USassociationsi

nvolvingAandBchangedduringconditioningwiththeABcompound(seeFigure10).InspectionofPanelAinFigure10con rmsthatVA�USandVB�USincreasedbyequivalentamounts,andthat 5ThesesimulationsareimplementedintheHeiDIappas\Poolederrorterm".84 whileVUS�Aremainedthesame,VUS�Bincreasedto.30.PanelBinFigure10showshowtheassociativestrengthsofADandBCchangewhenthechangesinvolvingAandBwereaddedtotheexistingstrengthsofDandB,respectively.InspectionofPanelBinFigure10con rmsthatthenetVAD�USandVBC�USincreaseequivalentlyasaconsequenceofABconditioningtrials(theblacksymbolsoverlapwithoneanother).However,whileVUS�BCincreases,VUS�ADdoesnot.PanelCinFigure10showsthattheVCOMB�BCisgreaterthanVCOMB�AD,re ectingthegreatercontributionofVUS�BCtoBCthanVUS�ADtoAD.Finally,PanelDinFigure10revealsthatthedi erencebetweenBCandADisevidentinbothRUSandRCS;butinabsolutetermsismostevidentforRCS.Thisdi erencere ectsthefactthatwiththeparametersemployedintheillustrativesimulation,thecombinedalphascores( ADand BC=.60)aregreaterthantheVAD�USandVBC�US(both=.25).Whenotheraspectsofthesimulationareheldconstant,but forallstimuliwassetat.10(i.e., ADand BC=.20),theabsolutedi erencebetweenBCandADis(approximately)equallyevidentforRUSandRCS.RescorlaandWagner(1972)isunabletoaccountforthefactthatreinforcementofacompound(AB+)a ectsdi erentlyapreviousexcitor(A)oraninhibitor(B)withoutweakeningassumptionsgoverningthelearningrulesoraddingmorecomplexitytothemodel.HeiDIcombinesapoolederrortermfortheforwardassociationsandseparateerrortermsforbackwardsassociations.Thisway,HeiDIisabletoaccommodatedi ere

ntialincreasesordecreasesinassociativestrengthforexcitorsandinhibitors,whichmodelsbasedonpurelycombinederrortermareunabletoaccountfor.3.6.4Combiningstimuliwithdi erentassociativehistoriesRescorlaandWagner(1972)madethesimplifyingassumptionthattheassociativestrengthofacompoundstimulus(VAB�US)issimplythesumoftheindividualassocia-tivestrengthsofAandB(i.e.,VA�US+VB�US).TogetherwiththeassumptionthatVbearsanordinalrelationshiptoperformance,themodelisconstrainedtopredictedthattherewillbeanordinalrelationshipbetweenperformancetoA,BandAB.Forexam-85 Figure10:SimulationsforHeiDIofthechangesinassociativestrengthwhencombiningstimuliwithdi erentassociativehistories.Aconditionedexcitor(A)andaninhibitor(B)areconditionedincompound(AB),andtestedwithaninhibitor(D)andexcitor(C)incompoundsADandBC.PanelAshowstheoutputvaluesforchangesinassociativestrengthofthecomponents(AandB)ofastimuluscompound(AB)thatispairedwithaUS.StimulusA(andC)begincompoundconditioningwithaVCS�USof.50,andVUS�CSof.30;whereasB(andD)beginwithaVCS�USof-.50andVUS�CSof0.PanelBdepictstheoutputvaluesforthetestcompounds:VAD�US,VUS�AD,VBC�USandVUS�BC.PanelCshowstheoutputvaluesforthecombinationoftheforwardandbackwardassociationsforAD(VCOMB�AD)andBC(VCOMB�BC),whilepanelDillustrateshowthedi erencesinVCOMB�ADandVCOMB�BCarere ectedintheoutputvaluesforRCS(CS-orientedbehaviour)andRUS(US-orientedbehaviour)duringthetestcompoundsADandBC.ple,iftwostimuliwithexcitatoryassociativestrengtharecombinedthenperformancetothecompoundABshouldexceedbothAandB;whereasifonestimulusisexcitatory86 (A)andtheother(B)isuntrained(andwithoutassociat

ivestrength)thenperformancetoABshouldmatchA,andbothshouldexceedB.Finally,ifAisexcitatoryandBinhibitorythenperformancetoABshouldbelessthanAandgreaterthanB,unlesstheexcitatoryvalueofAwaslessthanorequaltotheinhibitoryvalueofB.WhilethepredictionsofHeiDIandtheRescorla-Wagnermodelmirroroneanotherinsomeofthesecases,theydivergeinothers.SummationTheanalysisbeginswiththe rstexample,wheretwoCSs(AandB)thathavebeenseparatelypairedwithUSarepredictedtosummatewhentheyarecombinedattest.Equations1and2wereusedtogeneratetherequisiteindividualVsforstimulusAandB,andEquations4and5todetermineperformance.First,itwascon rmedthatsummationwasevidentinbothRCSandRUSirrespectiveofwhethertheparameterswerechosentoresultinabiastowardstheRCS(e.g., Aand B=.50,and US=.30),orRUS(e.g., Aand B=.50,and US=.70).However,atanempiricallevel,summationisnotaninevitableconsequenceofpresentingtwoexcitatorystimuliincom-pound.Thecircumstancesunderwhichsummationdoesanddoesnotoccurhaveyettobefullydetermined(Pearce,Aydin,&Redhead,1997;Pearce,Redhead,&George,2002),withtheoreticalanalysestendingtofocusonhowthecombinationorcon gura-tionofstimulichangesthewayinwhichtheyareprocessed(e.g.,Brandon,Vogel,&Wagner,2000;Pearce,1994).Iwillnotcommentfurtherhereonthenatureofsuch\con gural"processes,buttheaforementionedtheoreticalanalysesmakeanimportantassumption:SeparateconditioningtrialswithAandBresultsinthemacquiringassocia-tivestrength(relatively)independentlyofoneanother(seeBrandonetal.,2000;Pearce,1994).HeiDIdoesnotmakethisassumption,andthisfacthasimportantimplicationsfortheconditionsunderwhichsummationwillbeobserved.HeiDIassumesthatas

sociationsformfromtheUStotheCS.UnlikethedevelopmentofA-USandB-USassociations,whichproceedsindependently,thenetUS-Aassociation87 willbeweakenedonatrialonwhichBispairedwiththeUSandthenetUS-BassociationwillbeweakenedonatrialwhenAispairedwiththeUS.Thisfactdoesnotinitselfa ectthepredictionthatsummationwillbeobserved(simulationsincludedthesereciprocalassociations),howeveritdoesraisethepossibilitythatanotherformoflearningwilloccurthatcouldconstrainsummation.TotheextentthattheA-USandUS-BassociationsenabletheBnodetobecomeactiveonatrialwithA,andtheB-USandUS-AenabletheAnodetobecomeactiveonatrialwithB,thereisthepotentialforinhibitiontodevelopbetweenAandB(seeMcLaren,Kaye,&Mackintosh,1989;McLaren&Mackintosh,2000).VA�B= A(c B�XVTotal�B)(7)VB�A= B(c A�XVTotal�A)(8)FirstitisassumedthatthechangeinthestrengthoftheassociationbetweennodesAandBisgovernedbyEquation7,andthereciprocalB-AassociationisgovernedbyEquation8.TheseequationsareformallyequivalenttoEquations1and2.Theyprovideabasisfortheformationofassociationsbetweentheelementsofacompound(AB),allowingbehaviourestablishedtoonestimulus(e.g.,A)totransfertotheother(e.g.,B).IwillreturntotheseCS-CSassociationsinthecontextofapotentialanalysisoffeaturesofblocking.TheequationsalsoprovidethebasisforthedevelopmentofinhibitionbetweenAandBwhenbothhavebeenpairedwiththesameUS.Accord-ingtoEquations7and8,netinhibitionwilldevelopbetweentheAandBnodes,totheextentthatthecombinede ectoftheforward(e.g.,A-US)andbackwardassocia-tions(e.g.,US-B)provideanindirectbasisforVA�BtobepositivewhenBisabsent.Thus,onatrialwhenAispresented, B=0andtheabili

tyofAtoactivateB(i.e.,VA�B)willdependonmultiplyingthestrengthsoftheA-USandUS-Bassociations:88 1/c*VA�US*VUS�B;andonatrialwhenBispresented, A=0andVB�Awilldependon:1/c*VB�US*VUS�A.ThedevelopmentofthisinhibitionwillmeanthatwhenAandBarepresentedtogether(e.g.,forasummationtest)theircorrespondingnodeswillbelesslikelytobecomeactivethanifeitherhadbeenpresentedalone:PerformancetoanABcompoundwillbeconstrainedbythedevelopmentofinhibitionbetweenthenodesactivatedbyAandBwhenbotharefollowedbythesameUS.ItisworthnotingthatsuchaconstraintonsummationwouldbelesslikelyifAandBweretobefol-lowedbydi erentreinforcersduringconditioning;reinforcerswiththesametendencytoprovokeconditionedrespondingbutwithdistinctsensoryproperties(e.g.,A-foodandB-sucrose).Inkeepingwiththeanalysisoutlinedinthepreviousparagraph(i.e.,thatHeiDIpredictssummationbetweentwostimuliseparatelypairedwiththesameUSwillbeconstrainedbyinhibitionbetweenthem),WattandHoney(1997)observedthatacom-pound(AB)wasmorelikelytoprovokeconditionedrespondingattestifitscomponentshadbeenseparatelypairedwithdi erentappetitivereinforcers(foodandsucrose)thatsupportthesameconditionedresponse,thaniftheyhadbeenpairedwiththesamereinforcer(foodorsucrose;orbothfoodandsucrose,ondi erenttrials).ItpredictsthisbecausethereislessinhibitionbetweenAandBwhentheyarefollowedbydi erentappetitiveUSsthanwhentheyarefollowedbythesameappetitiveUS:ThesharedsensorypropertiesofthesharedUSsprovideabasisforinhibitionbetweenAandB,andthismeansthattheycansuppressoneanother'sactivation.Ingeneralterms,di er-encesinthedevelopmentofinhibitionbetweenAandBengenderedbydi erenttraining

proceduresshoulda ectthelikelihoodofsummationbeingobserved.ThedevelopmentofinhibitionbetweenAandB,whenbotharepairedwiththesameoutcome,hasnotbeendirectlyassessedinstudiesofsummationorconsideredatatheoreticallevel(cf.Brandon,Vogel,&Wagner,2000;Pearce,1994).However,thereisevidencethatiscon-sistentwiththissuggestionfromstudiesofcategorization(Aitken,Bennett,McLaren,&Mackintosh,1996)andperceptuallearning(e.g.,Dwyer&Mackintosh,2002;Mundy,89 Dwyer,&Honey,2006).ExternalinhibitionWhenanassociativelyneutralstimulus(B)ispresentedwithastimuluswithassocia-tivestrength(A)theconditionedresponsetothatstimulusisoftendisrupted;ane ectknownasexternalinhibition.Forexample,Pavlov(1927,p.44)originallyobservedthattheamountofconditionedrespondingtoaCS(inhiscasetheamountofsaliva-tionindogs)wasreducedwhenastimuluswithnoassociativepropertieswaspresentedwiththeCS.Thise ectisnotpredictedbytheRescorla-Wagnermodel,andhasbeeninterpretedintermsofadecreaseinattentiontotheCS(Mackintosh,1974,p.16).InasetofsimulationsinwhichtheassociativestrengthofVB�USwassettozeroanditwaspresentedwithastimuluswithexcitatoryassociativestrength(A)thatpossessedexcitatoryassociativestrength(VA�US�0),thepresenceofBincreasedRCSandre-ducedRUS,forABrelativetoA;whichwouldincreaseCS-orientedbehaviourrelativetoUS-orientedbehaviour.Thereisalsosomeevidencethatisconsistentwiththisanalysisfromstudiesofarelatede ect,knownasdisinhibition,whereconditionedresponding(e.g.,instrumentalleverpressingforfood)canbeaugmentedbythepresentationofastimulus(e.g.,alightorwhitenoiseBrimer,1970;Brimer&Kamin,1963).Infact,thise ectappearstobemostapparentwhenthelevelof

leverpressingislow(e.g.,attheonsetofa xedinterval,Flanagan&Webb,1964;Hinrichs,1968).Unfortunately,noneofthesestudiesmeasuredongoinggoal-tracking,whichshouldbethemirrorimageofbehaviourdirectedtowardsthelever.SummationtestsforconditionedinhibitionFinally,combiningastimuluswithstrongexcitatoryproperties(A)andastimuluswithmodestnetinhibitoryproperties(B)willmeanthatVAB�USwilltakealowervaluethanVA�US.Equations1and2wereusedtogeneratetheindividualVsforareinforcedstimulus(A)andastimulus(B)thatwasnon-reinforcedinthepresenceofA.Equations90 3-5wereusedtodetermineperformance.WhethertheparameterswerechosentoresultinabiastowardsRCS(e.g., Aand B=.50,and US=.30),orRUS(e.g., Aand B=.50,and US=.70),combiningAwithBresultedinlowerlevelsofboth.ThevaluesforRCSandRUSfortheABcompoundwouldremainpositive(albeitlowerthanthoseforAalone)becauseVCOMBwillstillbepositive.However,ifAhadmodestexcitatorypropertiesandBhadstronginhibitoryproperties,thenVCOMBwouldbenegative-andasaresultRCSandRUSwouldalsobenegative.AdoptingEquation6wouldmeanthatr1wouldbenegative(unlesseitherCS-r1orUS-r1werealsonegative).Inthiscase,anexampleofapositiver1mightbetoapproachtheleverandanegativer1towithdrawfromthelever.IfthenegativevaluesreturnedbyEquations1and2wereconstruedasinvolvingtheactivationofaNoUSnode(Konorski,1967;Pearce&Hall,1980),thentheexcitatoryCS-NoUSassociationwouldresultinRCSandRNO�USbeingpositive,andRNO�UScouldthendirectlygeneratedi erentformsofrespondingnotsupportedbyeithertheCSorUS.3.6.5Blocking:LearningandperformanceItwasnotedintheintroductionthatoneofthekeyfeaturesoftheRescorla-Wagnermodelwasitsabilitytoexplai

nhowtheassociativestrengthofonestimuluswithinacompounda ectstheassociativestrengthgainedbyanotherstimuluswithinthecom-pound(e.g.,blocking,Kamin,1969).TheformalsimilaritybetweenEquation1andtheRescorla-Wagnermodelisclear,andlikethismodel,Equation1generatestheseimpor-tante ectsonthedevelopmentoftheCS-USassociation.However,otherfeaturesofHeiDImeanthatblockingisnot{astheRescorla-Wagnermodelpredicts{inevitable.Inextremis,Equations1-3inconcertwithEquations4and5provideanaccountofblockingthatisclearlyrelatedtotheRescorla-Wagnermodel:IfVA�USc* USattheendofaperiodoftrainingwhereAhasbeenpairedwithaUS,thenconditioningwithacompound(AB)willresultinlittleornoincreaseintheB-USassociation(i.e.,VB�US0).However,accordingtoHeiDI,thereciprocalUS-Bassociation(VUS�B)willbe91 una ectedbythefactthatAhasareciprocalassociationwiththeUS(VUS�A),becausethec* Aandc* BvaluesofAandBprovideaseparatebasisfortheformationoftheseassociations.ThepredictionthattheUS-BassociationisnotblockedwillordinarilybewithoutconsequencebecauseEquation3willreturnaVCOMBforB0(i.e.,ifVB�US0thenVB�US+(1/c*VB�US*VUS�B)0).AccordingtoEquations4and5,RCSandRUS0becauseVCOMB0.However,oneclearimplicationofthisanalysisisthattreatmentsthatenabletheUS-Bassociationtoin uenceperformanceshouldreducetheblockinge ect;andthereisevidencethattheperformancetoablockedstimuluscanbeaugmentedundersomeconditions(forareview,seeMilleretal.,1995).BothHeiDIandtheRescorla-WagnermodelpredictthatVB�US(andVA�US)willincreaseduringthecompoundconditioningphaseofablockingprocedureifVA�USc* US.However,unliketh

eRescorla-Wagnermodel,HeiDIpredictsthatthepatternofperformancewhenBistestedwillre ectthevaluesofc* BandVB�US.Undertheseconditions,AmightgenerateUS-orientedbehaviour(i.e.,VA�US&#x]TJ/;༕ ;.9;Ւ ;&#xTf -;е.;व ;&#x-23.;ई ;&#xTd [;c* A),buttheassociativestrengthgainedbyBmightbeevidentasCS-orientedbehaviour(i.e.,c* B&#x]TJ/;༕ ;.9;Ւ ;&#xTf -;е.;व ;&#x-23.;ई ;&#xTd [;VB�US).Thissimpleobservationhasanimportantimplication:Ablockinge ectmightnotbeevidentiftheexperimentalassaywasmoresensitivetoCS-orientedbehaviourthantoUS-orientedbehaviour.ThefactthatVB�USislowwillreduceVCOMBinEquation3,butitwillsimultaneouslyincreasethecontributiontoperformanceoftheCS-orientedcomponent(i.e.,RCS)andreducetheUS-orientedcomponent(i.e.,RUS).Whileitwouldbetendentioustoarguethatfailurestoobserveblocking(e.g.,Maesetal.,2016)providesupportfortheanalysispresentedabove{groundsforsuchfailuresabound{therecanlittledoubtthatblockinge ectscanbelesscompletethanasimplerenderingoftheRescorla-Wagnermodelwouldpredict(forarecentreviewandanalysis,seeUrcelay,2017).However,perhapsthemostseriouschallengetotheaccountofblockingo eredbytheRescorla-Wagnermodelinvolvestheconditionsunderwhich\unblocking"occurs.Conventionalproceduresforblockinginvolvetwostagesinwhichthereinforceristhe92 same:A-�USandthenAB-�US.ThefactthatincreasingthenumberofUSsbetweenstage1(e.g.,A-�US1)andstage2(AB-�US1-US2)resultsinunblocking(i.e.,learningaboutB)isperfectlyconsistentwiththemodel,becausethischangeintroducesapositivediscrepancyinthepoolederrorterm(seeEquations0and1).T

heproblematicresultisthefactthatreducingthereinforcer(i.e.,A-�US1-US2andthenAB-�US1)canalsoresultinrespondingtoB(i.e.,unblocking,Dickinson,Hall,&Mackintosh,1976).Takeninisolation,Equations0and1predictthatthereductioninthenumberofreinforcersshouldhaveresultedinBacquiringinhibitoryproperties(e.g.,Cottonetal.,1982).`Downshiftunblocking',asitisknown,hasbeentakenasevidencethatthereductionintheUSpreventsthereductioninattentiontoBthatwouldordinarilyresultfromthefactthattheUSwaspredictedbyA;andallowsBtobelearntabout(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980).Whiletherehasbeensomeprogressinunderstandingtheconditionsunderwhichdownshiftunblockingoccurs(Holland,1988)thereisnoconsensusaboutitsexplanation.Manyhavesimplyadoptedtheviewthatdownshiftunblockingisprimafacieevidencethatattentioncanchangeasaresultofexperience(Pearce&Mackintosh,2010).However,aspeculativeexplanationforthise ectcanbederivedfromapplicationofHeiDI,withoutappealingtochangesinattention(seeFigure11).Theessenceoftheanalysisisthattheremovalofthesecondshockallowsawithin-compoundB-Aassociationtoformmoree ectivelyduringdownshiftunblockingthanduringstandardblocking;andthisassociationallowsBto\borrow"theassociativepropertiesofA.ConsiderablockingprocedureinwhichAis rstfollowedbysuccessivepresentationsofthesamenominalUS.EachUScanbetreatedashavingpartiallyseparaterepresentations(US1andUS2).Undertheseconditions,AwillbecomelinkedtobothUS1andUS2untileachlinkreachestheasymptotedeterminedbyc* US1andc* US2;andcriticallylinkswillbestrengthenedbetweenUS1andA,andUS2andA,untiltheircombinedassociativestrength=c* A.WhenABispairedwithUS1andUS2,thea

ssociationsbetweenBandbothUS1andUS2willbeblocked;andthe93 Figure11:SimulationsforHeiDIofthechangesinassociativestrengthduringstandardblocking(panelsA-C)anddownshiftunblockingprocedures(panelsD-E).Theparam-etersusedwere: A= B=.30,and US1= US2=.30.Attheoutsetofcompoundconditioning,A-US1andA-US2weresetto.30,andUS1-AandUS2-Awerebothsetto.15.PanelsAandDshowtheoutputvaluesforthestrengthsoftheAandBwithUS1associations(A-US1,US1-A,B-US1,US1-B)andPanelsBandEshowtheoutputvaluesforthestrengthsoftheAandBwithUS2associations(A-US2,US2-A,B-US2,US2-B),returnedbyEquations1and2combinedwithEquations7and8.NotethatUS2-AandUS2-Bissetto0inpanelEtore ectthefactthattheUS2isabsent;buttheseassociationswillnotchangeduringunblocking.PanelsCandFshowthestrengthoftheA-BandB-Aassociations.AkeyobservationisthattheB-Aassociationgainsstrengthduringdownshiftunblocking(panelF),butnotstandardblocking(panelC).combinede ectoftheUS1-AandUS2-AassociationswillmeanthatBwillnotbeabletoenterassociationwithA.However,thiswillnotbethecasewhenUS2isomitted.IfthechangeintheB-Aassociationisassumedtobedeterminedby B*(c* A{PVTOTAL�A),withPVTOTAL�A=VUS1�A+VUS2�A+VB�A,thentheremovalofUS2willenablethestrengtheningoftheB-Aassociation(andfurtherincreasesintheUS1-Aassociation).Undertheseconditions,downshiftunblockingwilloccurtotheextentthatthein uenceoftheB-AassociationinretrievingtheassociativepropertiesofA(strongerfollowingdownshiftunblockingthanstandardblocking)outweighsthefact94 thatA-US2isweakerandB-US2isnegativeafterdownshiftunblocking.Thisaccountisnecessarilyspeculative,mirroringthefactthattheunderstandingoftheconditionsunder

whichdownshiftunblockingoccursremainsincomplete(seeHolland,1988).However,itreceivessupportfromstudiesconductedbyRescorlaandColwill(1983),whichshowedthatmanipulationsthatshoulddisruptB-Aassociationsalsoreducethedi erenceinperformancetoBbetweenstandardblockinganddownshiftunblocking6.Thesimulations7presentedinFigure11forthecompoundconditioningstagearebased{intheinterestsofsimplicity{onthefollowingparameters: A= B=c* A=c* B=.30,and US1= US2=c* US1=c* US2=.308.However,thecriticaldi erenceintheB-Aassociationduringstandardblockinganddownshiftunblockingisageneralone.Attheoutsetofsimulatedcompoundconditioning,forbothstandardblocking(panelsA-C)anddownshiftunblocking(panelsD-F),VA�US1wassetto.30andVA�US2wassetto.30tore ecttheassumptionthat US1= US2=c* US1=c* US2=.30.Critically,VUS1�AandVUS2�Aweresetat.15forstandardblocking,whereasfordownshiftunblockingVUS1�Awassetat.15andVUS2�Awassetto0(tore ectthefactthatUS2isabsent).Forthesamereason,VUS2�Bwasalsosetto0.PanelsA-C(standardblocking)andpanelsD-F(downshiftunblocking)depictthevaluesreturnedbythecombinationofEquations1and2withEquations7and8.PanelsAandDshowassociationsbetweenAandBwithUS1(A-US1,US1-A,B-US1,US1-B)andPanelsBandEshowassociationsbetweenAandBwithUS2(A-US2,US2-A,B-US2,US2-B)andPanelsCandFshowassociationsbetweenAandB(VA�Band 6Itisworthnotingthatwithin-compound(A-B)associationscouldalsoformduringtheexperimentsdemonstratingunequalchangeintheassociativestrengthoftheelementsofacompound(AB).However,inthiscase,therewasevidencethattheseassociationswerenotresponsibleforthee ectsthatwereobserved(s

eeAllmanetal.,2004;Rescorla,2000)7ThesesimulationsareimplementedintheHeiDIappas\Downshiftunblocking".8ThesimulationsthatIreportdonotincludeassociationsbetweenUS1andUS2,becausetheywouldnotin uencetheformationtheexcitatoryB-Aassociationuponwhichtheanalysisrests.Moreover,whiletheformationofUS2-US1andUS1-US2associationswouldtendtoreducerespectivelytheA-US1andA-US2associationsduringconditioningwithA,theabsenceofUS2duringdownshiftunblockingwouldallowincreasesinboththeA-US1andB-US1associations.Furthermore,thereductionsinthenetassociativestrengthoftheA-US2andB-US2associationsproducedbytheabsenceofUS2wouldbelessmarkedthanthosedepictedinFigure8Dand8E,becauseUS1wouldgainaproportiontheoverallnetreduction.Insummary,theinclusionofUS1-US2associationsincreasesthelikelihoodthatdownshiftunblockingwouldbeobserved.95 VB�A).InspectionofpanelsA-Ccon rmsthatduringstandardblockingassociationsinvolvingAremainedthesame,andthatassociationsfromUS1,US2andAtoBallincreasedbyequivalentamounts.Critically,theB-Aassociationdidnotdevelop,andthisassociationcanprovidenobasisuponwhichBcouldprovokeconditionedresponding;andthereciprocalUS1-BandUS2-Bassociationscannot{inisolation{contributetoperformance.Incontrast,duringdownshiftunblockingbecauseUS2isabsenttheUS1-AandB-Aassociationscanstrengthen.ThiswillmeanboththatVCOMB�AwillbehigherfollowingdownshiftunblockingthanstandardblockingandthatBwillbeabletoaccessVCOMB�AthroughtheB-Aassociation.Inorderforthisstateofa airstogeneratemoreperformancetoBitwouldneedtooutweighthefactthattheA-US2andB-US2areweakerorinhibitoryafterdownshiftunblockingthanstandardblocking.InChapter5,Iwillconsiderhowtheasso

ciativestrength(VCOMB�A)borrowedbyonestimulus(B)fromanotherstimulus(A),withwhichithasanassociation(VB�A),ismanifestinperformance.Fornow,itissucienttonotethatHeiDIprovidesoneformalanalysisofhowwithin-compoundassociationsmighta ecttheoutcomeofblockingandunblockingprocedures(cf.Urcelay,2017).3.6.6Latentinhibition:AnalternativeassociativeanalysisRescorlaandWagner(1972)recognizedthefactthatwhiletheirmodelprovidedareadyaccountforblocking,itdidnotaddressthefactthatsimplepreexposuretoaCSretardslaterexcitatoryandinhibitoryconditioning(forareview,seeHall,1991;Lubow,1989).Thatis,theoriginalmodeldidnotprovideanaccountoflatentinhibition(Lubow&Moore,1959).But,whyshouldrepeatedpresentationofato-be-conditionedstimulusa ecttherateatwhich(excitatoryandinhibitory)conditionedperformanceemergestothatstimulus?Thisobservationinparticular,aswellasdownshiftunblocking,haspromptedtheoriststoconcludethatmodelsofPavlovianconditioningneedtoincludeanotherprocessthatchangesasafunctionofexperience:attention,associabilityofCSprocessing(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Wagner,1981).96 However,acriticalfeatureoflatentinhibition,whichprovidesapotentialtheoreticallinkwithanassociativeanalysisofblocking,isthatlatentinhibitioniscontextspeci c.IfpreexposuretotheCSoccursinonecontext(de nedbythecuespresentinoneexperimentalchamber)andconditioningtakesplaceinanothercontext,thenlatentinhibitionismuchreduced(e.g.,Hall&Honey,1989;Honey&Good,1993).Thegeneralsigni canceofthisobservationisthatitsuggeststhat{duringthepreexposurestage{animalsencodewherethestimulushasbeenpresented;forexample,byformingacontext-CSassociation(cf.Wagn

er,1981).ThisobservationenablesHeiDItoprovideasimpleanalysisoflatentinhibition:theblockingoftheUS-CSassociationbythecontext-CSassociation9Itwasarguedthatduringexcitatoryconditioning,performanceisdeterminedbybothaCS-USassociationandaUS-CSassociation,andthatduringinhibitoryconditioning,performancecouldre ectthestatusofbothaCS-NoUSandaNoUS-CSassociation(Konorski,1967).Whileacontext-CSassociationwillnotblocktheCS-USandCS-NoUSassociations,itwillblockthedevelopmentoftheUS-CSandNoUS-CSassociations.Thus,thesimpleinclusionofaUS-CSassociation(andNoUS-CSassociation)enablesanaccountoflatentinhibitionthatdoesnotrequireaseparateattentionalorassociabilityprocess(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980)orchangesinCSprocessingoftheformenvisagedbyWagner(1981).Inadditiontothisnovelanalysisoflatentinhibition,thepresenceofaUS-CSassoci-ationmeansthatthee ectivesalienceofCSsthataregoodpredictorscanbeaugmented(cf.Mackintosh,1975).Ithasbeenshownthatthe valueofastimulusa ectstherateatwhichCS-orientedandUS-orientedcomponentsofperformancedevelop(seeFig-ure8).TheUS-CSassociationprovidesanaturalwayinwhichactivationoftheUSmightbere ectedbacktotheCSandmaintainitsactivation.Moreover,Ihavealready 9Itshouldbeacknowledgedthatwhilethecontextspeci cityoflatentinhibitionisconsistentwiththeviewthatcontext-CSassociationsprovideapotentialexplanationforlatentinhibition(andhabit-uation),thefactthatattemptstoextinguishthecontext-CSassociationhaveoftenhadnoe ectonlatentinhibitionisinconsistentwiththisaccount(seeBaker&Mercier,1982;Hall&Minor,1984).However,theinterpretationoffailuresofthiskindisnotstraightforward(seeHoney,Iordanova,&Good,2010

).97 notedthatwhenaCSisfollowedbyareductioninUSmagnitude(e.g.,duringextinc-tionorpartialreinforcement),CS-orientedrespondingincreasesrelativetoUS-orientedresponding,whichcouldalsoa ectthesubsequentlearninginvolvingthatCS.HeiDItherebyprovidesasimpleanalysisofphenomenathatareroutinelytakentoindicatethattheassociabilityofstimuli(their value)ortheirprocessingchangesasaresultofexperience(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Pearce&Mackintosh,2010;Wagner,1981).Accordingtothisanalysis,thesephenomenaareanotherproductofthereciprocalassociationsthatformbetweentheCSandUS,andbetweenthecomponentsofstimuluscompounds.InthischapterIexaminedhowHeiDIisabletoaccommodatesomeofphenomenathatarebeyondofthescopeofRescorla-WagnerModel,andhowlearningwouldtrans-lateintoconditionedbehaviourdirectedtowardstheCSortheUS.Inthefollowingchapter,Chapter4,Idiscussandtestnewpredictionsderivedfromthemodel.Chapter5,theGeneralDiscussion,willthenreturntothewidertheoreticalimplicationsoftheHeiDImodel.98 3.7ListofequationsVCS�US= CS(c US�XVTotal�US)(1)VUS�CS= US(c CS�XVTotal�CS)(2)VCOMB=VCS�US+(1 cVCS�USVUS�CS)(3a)VCOMB�AB=XVCS�US+[1 cXVAB�US(VUS�A+VUS�B)](3b)RCS=c CS c CS+jVCS�USjVCOMB(4)RUS=jVCS�USj c CS+jVCS�USjVCOMB(5)r1=(1 cRCSCSr1)+(1 cRUSUSr1)(6)VA�B= A(c B�XVTotal�B)(7)VB�A= B(c A�XVTotal�A)(8)99 Chapter4TestingpredictionsofHeiDI4.1Individualdi erencesin US4.1.1IntroductionAsdiscussedinChapter3(Section3.2),recentevidencesuggeststhatperformanceis

a ectedbyhowtheoutcomeisvaluedorperceived.Dwyeretal.(2018)showedthatindividualdi erencesinthepalatabilityofsucrose(duringtheirexperimentsin-volvingcontraste ects)werepositivelycorrelatedwiththe avourpreferencelearning.Moreover,rodentswhoshowedastronglikingforsucrose(asmeasuredbylickingmi-crostructure;seeDwyer,2012)aremorelikelytobegoal-trackers(whensucrosewastheUS)thanthosewhoexhibitedaweakerlikingforsucrose(Patituccietal.,2016).HeiDIassumesthattheperceivedsalienceoftheCS(relatingto CS)andUS(relat-ingto US;Equations4and5)variesbetweenanimals,howeverthevaluesof CSand USare xedforagivenanimal.ThisvariationinhowtheUSisvalueda ectsbothlearning(i.e.,theasymptoticvalueofVCS�USandtherateatwhichVUS�CSreachesasymptote,throughEquations1and2)andthedistributionofVCOMBinperformance(throughVCS�USinEquations3-6).SimulationsofHeiDIarepresentedinFigure13.TheparametersduringconditioningwerechosentoresulteitherinahigherUSvalue( CS=.30and US=.70;panelsAandB)oralowerUSvalue( CS=.30and US=.50;panelsCandD).Simulationforconditioning(lefthandsideofeachpanel;trials1-6)andextinction(right-handsideofeachpanel;trials7-12)forareinforced(+)lever.PanelsAandCdepicttheoutputvaluesforVCS-US,VUS-CS,VCOMB,andpanelsBandDshowthecorrespondingoutputvaluesforRCSandRUS.ItcanbeobservedfromFigure13thatahigherUSvaluewillresultinhigherlevelsofgoal-trackingorRUS(panelB),incontrastwithlowerUSvalue100 (PanelD).Notethat,incontrastwiththeanalysispresentedfordownshiftunblockinginChapter3(section3.6.5),atwopelletrewardispresentedasahigher USvalueinsteadoftwoseparate USvaluesforUS1andUS2.Toexploreth

isprediction,ratsweregiventrainingwithonereinforcedlever(e.g.,leftlever)andonenon-reinforcedlever(e.g.,rightlever).Halftheanimalswerereinforcedwithonepelletrewardandtheotherhalfwithtwopelletsreward,aftereachpresentationofthereinforcedlever.HeiDIpredictsthatthismanipulationwillbeshownintherat'stendencytointeractwiththefood-well,inotherwords,animalsinthetwopelletgroupwillhavehigheroverallgoal-trackinglevelsthananimalsintheonepelletgroup.4.1.2Experiment3MethodAnimalsandapparatusTwenty-threenaivemaleSpragueDawleyratswereused(suppliedbyCharlesRiver,UK).Theanimalsweresplitintotwogroups:oneortwopelletsreward(11ratsinonepelletgroup,12ratsinthetwopelletgroup).TheratswerehousedasdescribedinExperiment1.Theirmeanadlibitumweightbeforethestartoftheexperimentwas384g(range:391-577g)andtheyweremaintainedatbetween85and95%oftheseweightsbygivingthemrestrictedaccesstofoodattheendofeachday.Theratshadcontinuousaccesstowaterwhentheywereintheircages.TheexperimentalchamberswerethoseusedinExperiment1.ThedesignoftheexperimentissimilartoExperiment1,withtheadditionthatinExperiment3,halfoftheratsreceivedtwopelletsasareward,insteadofone(seeTable3).ProcedureTheprocedureusedforExperiment3wasthesameasforExperiment1withtheadditionofanotherexperimentalgroupwerethereinforcerwasatwopelletrewardinsteadofone.101 Table3:DesignOftheExperiment3and4 Classi cationTrainingReversalNotation 1pelL1+L1-L1(+k-)1pelL2-L2+L2(-k+)1pel 2pelL1+L1-L1(+k-)2pelL2-L2+L2(-k+)2pel Note:1peland2pelrefertotherewardmagnitude(onegroupofratswasreinforcedwithonepelletandtheothergroupwithtwopellets);L1andL2refertotwolevers(leftandright,counterbalanced).Duringtraining,L1wasreinf

orced(\+";onefoodpelletinthe1pelgrouportwofoodpelletsinthe2pelgroup)andL2wasnon-reinforced(\-").DuringthereversalstageL1wasnon-reinforcedandL2wasreinforced(oneortwopellets).DataAnalysisDataanalysiswasconductedasdescribedinExperiment1.RatsweresplitintoSTsandGTstakingamediansplit(asdescribedinExperiment1)foreachrewardgroup(oneandtwopellets).Forthe1pellet,groupbiasscoresabove.67wereclassi edasGTsandbelow.58asSTs.Forthetwopelletgroup,biasscoresabove.23wereclassi edasGTsandbiasscoresbelow.19asSTs.Thedistributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourisshowninFigure12.MixedANOVAswereconductedseparatelyforleverpressingandfood-wellentrieswithwithin-subjectsfactorsblock(training6levels:T1-T6;transitionblocks2levels:T6-R1;reversal6levels:R1-R6;)andlever(2levels:reinforced\+"vsnon-reinforced\-")andbetween-subjectsfactorclassi cation(2levels:STvsGT)asinExperiment1,withtheadditionofrewardasbetween-subjectsfactor(2levels:onevstwopellets).ResultsSimulationsofHeiDIarepresentedinFigure13.ThemainresultsfromExperiment3areshowninFigure14.Theanalysiswillbeginwithresultsfromthetrainingstage(lefthandpanelsofFigure14),beforemovingtothetransitionbetweentrainingandreversal(identi edbythegreysection),and nallythereversalstageasawhole(right-hand102 Figure12:Distributionofsign-tracking(leverpresses)andgoal-tracking(food-wellentries)behaviourforExperiment3per(10-s)trialduringlastblockoftraining(T6)forL1(+jj-).Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).pannels).TrainingInspectionoftheresultsfromthe rststageoftraining(left-handsideoftheupperandlowerpanelsofFigure14)suggestthatastra

iningprogressedratsinbothgroups(STandGT)showedmoreleverpressesandfood-wellentriesduringthereinforcedL1thanthenonreinforcedL2.Forleverpresses,bothSTsandGTsshowhigherlevelsofresponsefortwopelletsreward,howeverthedi erencewasnumericallylargerinSTsthanGTs.Food-wellentriesdidnotshowadi erencebetweenoneandtwopelletsrewardforeitherSTsorGTs.Thedescriptionofthetrainingresultsissupportedbyseparateanalysesofleverpressesandfood-wellentries.103 Figure13:SimulationofHeiDIforindividualdi erencesin USonsignandgoal-tracking:Simulationforconditioning(left-handsideofeachpanel;trials1-6)andex-tinction(right-handsideofeachpanel;trials7-12)forthereinforced(+)lever.PanelsAandCdepicttheoutputvaluesforVCS-US,VUS-CS,VCOMB,andpanelsBandDshowthecorrespondingoutputvaluesforRCSandRUS.TheparametersduringconditioningwerechosentoresulteitherinahigherUSvalue(i.e., CS=.30and US=.70;panelsAandB)oralowerUSvalue(i.e., CS=.30and US=.50;panelsCandD).Duringextinctionofreinforcement, USwassetto0.LeverpressesAnANOVAconductedforleverpressesrevealedmaine ectsofreward,F(1,19)=6.45,p=.020,2p=.25,classi cation,F(1,19)=40.41,p.001,2p=.68,block,F(2.43,46.20)=8.96,p.001,2p=.32,andlever,F(1,19)=65.83,p.001,2p=.78.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.43,46.20)=4.73,p=.009,2p=.20,andcriticallybetweenrewardandlever,F(1,19)=6.75,p=.018,2p=.26,aswellasclassi cationandlever,F(1,19)=45.42,p.001,2p=.71andbetweenblockandlever,F(2.36,44.78)=14.22,p.001,2p=.43,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,19)=2.28,104 Figure14:ResultsfromExperiment3:thee ectofdi

;erentvaluesof USonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Mean(+SEM)leverpresses(upperpanel)andfood-wellentries(lowerpanel)per(10-s)trialacrossthetwostages:training(T1{T6)andreversal(R1{R6).Duringtraining,ratsreceivedpresentationsofoneleverpairedwithfoodpellets(L1(+jj-))andnonreinforcedpresentationsofasecondlever(L2(-jj+));ratswereclassi edassign-trackers(STs)andgoal-trackers(GTs)onthebasisoftheirbehaviourduringthe nalblockoftraining(T6).Theythenreceivedareversal:L1non-reinforcedandL2reinforced.Thegreysectionindicatestransitionbetweeninitialtrainingandthereversalofthecontingencies.p=.147,2p=.11orbetweenrewardandblock,F(2.43,46.20)=0.52,p=.631,2p=.03.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.36,44.78)=6.83,p=.002,2p=.26,butnointeractionbetweenreward,classi -cationandblock,F(2.43,46.20)=0.67,p=.544,2p=.03,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,19)=2.18,p=.156,2p=.10,reward,blockandlever,F(2.36,44.78)=0.71,p=.520,2p=.04.Therewasnofour-wayinteraction,F(2.36,44.78)=0.70,p=.522,2p=.04.Food-wellentries105 AnANOVAconductedforfood-wellentriesrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,19)=7.16,p=.015,2p=.27,block,F(2.31,43.84)=16.86,p.001,2p=.47,lever,F(1,19)=43.07,p.001,2p=.69,butnotofreward,F(1,19)=0.26,p=.616,2p=.01.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi ca-tionandblock,F(2.31,43.84)=4.28,p=.016,2p=.18,andbetweenblockandlever,F(2.62,49.70)=14.28,p.001,2p=.43,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,19)=0.03,p=.855,2p.01,rewardandblock,F(2.31,43.84)=0.81,p=.468,2p=.04,betweenrewardandlever,F(1,19)=0.00,p=.950,

2p.01orbetweenclas-si cationandlever,F(1,19)=0.68,p=.419,2p=.03.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.62,49.70)=5.24,p=.005,2p=.22,butnotbetweenreward,classi cationandblock,F(2.31,43.84)=0.73,p=.505,2p=.04,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,19)=0.25,p=.625,2p=.01,orbetweenreward,blockandlever,F(2.62,49.70)=0.43,p=.708,2p=.02.Therewasnofour-wayinter-actionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(2.62,49.70)=0.60,p=.598,2p=.03.TransitionblocksInspectionoftheresultsforthetransitionblocks(greysectioninFigure14)forleverpressessuggestsnodi erencebetweenonepelletandtwopelletsrewardforeitherSTsorGTs(upperpanels).Thesameistrueforfood-wellentries,nodi erenceisobservedbetweentheoneandtwopelletsineitherSTsorGTs.Thereversalincontingenciesa ectedmorethelevelsoffood-wellentries(lowerpanels)thanlever-presses(upperpan-els)inbothgroups(1peland2pel).Thisdescriptionofthetrainingresultsissupportedbyseparateanalysesofleverpressesandfood-wellentries.UnlikeinExperiment1,thelargerandfastere ectofreversingcontingenciesforfood-wellentriesthanlever-presseswasadi erenceofdegree,asreversaldidimpactonleverpresstosomedegreeduringthetransitionblocks.LeverpressesAnANOVAconductedforleverpressesforthetransitionblocksrevealedmainef-106 fectsofclassi cation,F(1,19)=19.38,p.001,2p=.50,block,F(1,19)=4.57,p=.046,2p=.19,lever,F(1,19)=7.73,p=.012,2p=.29,butnotofreward,F(1,19)=1.15,p=.297,2p=.06.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi ca-tionandlever,F(1,19)=1.73,p=.204,2p=.08,andcriticallybetweenblockandlever,F(1,19)=43.37,p.0

01,2p=.70,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,19)=0.06,p=.813,2p.01,criticallyrewardandblock,F(1,19)=0.15,p=.702,2p.01,classi cationandblock,F(1,19)=3.79,p=.066,2p=.17,orrewardandlever,F(1,19)=0.80,p=.383,2p=.04.Therewerenotripleinter-action,reward,classi cationandblock,F(1,19)=0.00,p=.996,2p.01,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,19)=0.16,p=.697,2p.01,reward,blockandlever,F(1,19)=1.18,p=.292,2p=.06,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1,19)=23.69,p.001,2p=.55.Therewasnofour-wayinteraction,reward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1,19)=0.16,p=.695,2p.01.Food-wellentriesAnANOVAconductedforfood-wellentriesrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,19)=5.67,p=.028,2p=.23,andlever,F(1,19)=45.03,p.001,2p=.70,butnotofreward,F(1,19)=3.92,p=.062,2p=.17orblock,F(1,19)=0.49,p=.494,2p=.02.Critically,therewereinteractionsbetweenrewardandblock,F(1,19)=5.82,p=.026,2p=.23,classi cationandblock,F(1,19)=7.05,p=.016,2p=.27,classi- cationandlever,F(1,19)=1.81,p=.195,2p=.09,andcriticallybetweenblockandlever,F(1,19)=59.50,p.001,2p=.76,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,19)=0.00,p=.974,2p.01,orbetweenrewardandlever,F(1,19)=0.37,p=.552,2p=.02.Therewasnotripleinteraction,reward,classi cationandblock,F(1,19)=0.55,p=.467,2p=.03,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,19)=0.14,p=.713,2p.01,reward,blockandlever,F(1,19)=0.20,p=.660,2p=.01,classi- cation,blockandlever,F(1,19)=6.91,p=.017,2p=.27orafour-wayinteractionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1,19)=0.24,p=.631,2

p=.01.IncontrasttoExperiment1andExperiment2therewasaninteractionbetweenblockand107 leverforleverpresses(F(1,19)=43.37,p.001,2p=.70),howeverthee ectsizeforfood-wellentriesisconsiderablylarger(F(1,19)=59.50,p.001,2p=.76).ReversalAcrosstheblocksofreversaltraining(right-handsideoftheupperandlowerpanelsofFigure14),nodi erenceswereobservedbetweentheoneandtwopelletgroupsinneitherleverpressesnorfood-wellentries.BothSTsandGTsincreasedrespondingforL2anddecreasedforL1forbothleverpressesandfood-wellentries.LeverpressesAnANOVAconductedforleverpressesforthereversalstagere-vealednomaine ectsofreward,F(1,19)=0.21,p=.653,2p=.01,classi cation,F(1,19)=0.62,p=.440,2p=.03,block,F(1.52,28.84)=1.58,p=.225,2p=.08,orlever,F(1,19)=0.68,p=.420,2p=.03.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi ca-tionandblock,F(1.52,28.84)=3.45,p=.057,2p=.15andbetweenblockandlever,F(1.29,24.60)=16.44,p.001,2p=.46,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,19)=0.00,p=.997,2p.01,rewardandblock,F(1.52,28.84)=1.30,p=.281,2p=.06,betweenrewardandlever,F(1,19)=0.69,p=.415,2p=.04orbetweenclas-si cationandlever,F(1,19)=1.25,p=.278,2p=.06.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(1.29,24.60)=3.10,p=.082,2p=.14,butnotbetweenreward,classi cationandblock,F(1.52,28.84)=0.09,p=.867,2p.01,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,19)=0.05,p=.822,2p.01orbetweenreward,blockandlever,F(1.29,24.60)=1.01,p=.347,2p=.05.Therewasnofour-wayinter-actionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1.29,24.60)=0.38,p=.595,2p=.02.Food-wellentriesAnANOVAconduct

edforfood-wellentriesforthereversalstagerevealedamainef-fectoflever,F(1,19)=12.41,p=.002,2p=.40,butnoe ectofreward,F(1,19)=1.75,p=.202,2p=.08,classi cation,F(1,19)=0.18,p=.674,2p.01orblock,F(2.51,47.72)=1.20,p=.318,2p=.06.Critically,therewasaninteractionbe-tweenblockandlever,F(2.08,39.60)=26.60,p.001,2p=.58,butnointeractions108 betweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,19)=0.16,p=.694,2p.01,rewardandblock,F(2.51,47.72)=2.27,p=.102,2p=.11,classi cationandblock,F(2.51,47.72)=1.48,p=.237,2p=.07,betweenrewardandlever,F(1,19)=0.23,p=.635,2p=.01,orbe-tweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,19)=0.08,p=.775,2p.01.Therewerenotripleinteractionsbetween,reward,classi cationandblock,F(2.51,47.72)=0.64,p=.568,2p=.03,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,19)=1.49,p=.237,2p=.07,reward,blockandlever,F(2.08,39.60)=0.09,p=.919,2p.01,classi cation,blockandlever,F(2.08,39.60)=2.31,p=.111,2p=.11orfour-wayinteractionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(2.08,39.60)=0.87,p=.432,2p=.04.DiscussionThepurposeofthisexperimentwastoexplorehowreinforcersofdi erentvaluesa ectconditionedbehaviour.HeiDIpredictshigherlevelsofUS-orientedbehaviourwithUSsofhighervalue.Incontrasttothisprediction,theresultsshowedthataUSwithahigher USvalue(twopellets)producedmoresign-trackingcomparedtoaUSwithalower USvalue(onepellet),andnodi erenceswereobservedingoal-tracking.Thismaybetheresultofmanipulatingrewardvaluethroughincreasingthenumberofrewardsratherthantheirsizeorvaluemoredirectly.Regardless,theresultsfromExperiment3,providefurtherevidencefortheres

ultsobtainedinExperiment1andExperiment2,replicatingthe ndingthatleverpressingbehaviourismoreresistanttocontingencychangesthanfood-wellbehaviour.4.1.3Experiment4WhileExperiment3providedresultsthatwerenotconsistentwiththepredictionsoftheHeiDImodelregardingthee ectsofincreasingthevalueoftheUS.Experiment4repeatedthesameexperimentaldesign.109 MethodAnimalsandapparatusTwenty-fourSpragueDawlleyfemaleratswereused(suppliedbyCharlesRiver,UK).TheratswerehousedasdescribedinExperiment1.Theirmeanadlibitumweightbeforethestartoftheexperimentwas238.17(range:205-309g)andtheyweremaintainedatbetween85and95%oftheseweightsbygivingthemrestrictedaccesstofoodattheendofeachday.Theratshadcontinuousaccesstowaterwhentheywereintheircages.TheapparatusisthesameasdescribedinExperiment1.ProcedureThesameprocedurewasusedasinExperiment3.DataanalysisThedatawasanalysedinthesamewayasinExperiment3.RatsweresplitintoSTsandGTstakingamediansplit(asdescribedinExperiment1)foreachrewardgroup(oneandtwopellets).Thedistributionoflever-pressesandfood-wellentriesbehaviourisshowninFigure16whereitisclearthatveryfewratspressedthelever.Fortheonepelletgroupbiasscoresbelow.99wereclassi edasSTandallscoresof1wereclassi edasGT.Themediansplitinthisexperimentmeantthatsomeanimalswereclassi edasSTswhentheywerebarelypressingthelever.Forthetwopelletgroupbiasscoresbelow.66wereclassi edasSTsandscoresabove.86asGTs.Thisimpliesadistributionskewedtowardsgoal-trackingbutlesssothanfortheonepelletgroup.ResultsTrainingInspectionoftheresultsfromthe rststageoftraining(left-handsideoftheupperandlowerpanelsofFigure15)suggeststhatonlytheanimalsinSTgrouparepressing110

Figure15:ResultsfromExperiment4(replicationofExperiment3infemalerats):thee ectofdi erentvaluesof USonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Mean(+SEM)leverpresses(upperpanel)andfood-wellentries(lowerpanel)per(10-s)trialacrossthetwostages:training(T1{T6)andreversal(R1{R6).Duringtraining,ratsreceivedpresenta-tionsofoneleverpairedwithfoodpellets(L1(+jj-))andnonreinforcedpresentationsofasecondlever(L2(-jj+));ratswereclassi edassign-trackers(STs)andgoal-trackers(GTs)onthebasisoftheirbehaviourduringthe nalblockoftraining(T6).Theythenreceivedareversal:L1non-reinforcedandL2reinforced.Thegreysectionindicatestransitionbetweeninitialtrainingandthereversalofthecontingencies.thelever,withnodi erencesbetweenoneortwopelletsasareinforcer.Incontrastwithpreviousexperiments,theGTanimalsshownolever-orientedbehaviour.Thedistributionoflever-pressesandfood-wellentriesbehaviourshowninFigure16suggeststhatcomparedtoothercohorts,relativelyfewanimalsshowleverpressbehaviourandtheGTgroupdisplayespeciallylittleleverpressbehaviour.Theimplicationsofthisissueareaddressedinthediscussionsection.Di erencesinthemagnitudeofrewardwerenotobservedineitherleverpressesorfood-wellentries.Thisdescriptionofthetrainingresultsissupportedbyseparateanalysesofleverpressesandfood-wellentries.111 Figure16:Distributionofsign-tracking(leverpresses)andgoal-tracking(food-wellentries)behaviourforExperiment4per(10-s)trialduringlastblockoftraining(T6)forL1(+jj-).Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).LeverpressesAnANOVAconductedforleverpressesforthetrainingstagerevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,44)=

0.02,p=.888,2p.01,block,F(2.46,108.12)=0.55,p=.616,2p=.01,lever,F(1,44)=0.14,p=.711,2p.01,butcriticallynoef-fectofreward,F(1,44)=2.16,p=.148,2p=.05.Therewasatwo-wayinteractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.46,108.12)=0.31,p=.777,2p.01,classi -cationandlever,F(1,44)=0.16,p=.691,2p.01,aswellasbetweenblockandlever,F(2.58,113.36)=0.46,p=.683,2p=.01,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi- cation,F(1,44)=2.46,p=.124,2p=.05,andcriticallynotbetweenrewardandblock,F(2.46,108.12)=3.26,p=.033,2p=.07orrewardandlever,F(1,44)=2.86,p=.098,2p=.06.Therewasathree-wayinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.58,113.36)=0.38,p=.740,2p.01,butnotbetweenreward,clas-si cationandblock,F(2.46,108.12)=2.57,p=.070,2p=.06,reward,classi cation112 andlever,F(1,44)=3.08,p=.086,2p=.07orbetweenreward,blockandlever,F(2.58,113.36)=3.46,p=.024,2p=.07.Therewasnofour-wayinteractionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(2.58,113.36)=3.09,p=.037,2p=.07.Food-wellentriesParallelanalysisconductedforfood-wellentriesforthetrainingstagerevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,44)=0.01,p=.913,2p.01andlever,F(1,44)=4.69,p=.036,2p=.10,butnoe ectofreward,F(1,44)=0.40,p=.528,2p.01orblock,F(2.47,108.72)=1.64,p=.192,2p=.04.Critically,therewasaninteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(2.58,113.43)=2.07,p=.117,2p=.04,butnoothertwo-wayinteractionsbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,44)=1.41,p=.241,2p=.03,criticallyrewardandblock,F(2.47,108.72)=0.55,p=.615,2p=.01,classi cationandblock,F(2.47,108.72)=1.33,p=.269,2p=.03

,rewardandlever,F(1,44)=2.78,p=.102,2p=.06,orclassi cationandlever,F(1,44)=0.82,p=.370,2p=.02.Therewerenothree-wayinteractionsbetweenreward,classi cationandblock,F(2.47,108.72)=0.93,p=.414,2p=.02,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,44)=3.36,p=.073,2p=.07,reward,blockandlever,F(2.58,113.43)=1.83,p=.154,2p=.04,classi cation,blockandlever,F(2.58,113.43)=0.60,p=.593,2p=.01,orafour-wayinteractionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(2.58,113.43)=1.78,p=.162,2p=.04.TransitionblocksInspectionoftheresultsforthetransitionblocks(greysectionofFigure15)showthatleverpresses(whentheyareatmateriallevels)arelessa ectedbythechangeincontingenciesthanfood-wellentries.LeverpressesANOVAconductedforthetransitionblocksforleverpressesrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,44)=0.07,p=.792,2p.01,andlever,F(1,44)=0.01,p=.906,2p.01,butnotofreward,F(1,44)=3.51,p=.068,2p=.07,orblock,F(1,44)=0.20,p=.654,2p.01.Therewasatwo-wayinteractionbetweenclassi-113 cationandlever,F(1,44)=0.01,p=.910,2p.01,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,44)=1.75,p=.193,2p=.04,rewardandblock,F(1,44)=0.71,p=.405,2p=.02,classi cationandblock,F(1,44)=0.04,p=.852,2p.01,rewardandlever,F(1,44)=2.63,p=.112,2p=.06orcriticallybetweenblockandlever,F(1,44)=0.10,p=.752,2p.01.Therewerenothree-waybetweeninteractionsre-ward,classi cationandblock,F(1,44)=0.27,p=.604,2p.01,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,44)=3.20,p=.080,2p=.07,reward,blockandlever,F(1,44)=0.81,p=.374,2p=.02,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1,44)=0.00,p=.955,

7;2p.01orafour-wayinteractionreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1,44)=0.04,p=.842,2p.01.Food-wellentriesANOVAconductedforthetransitionblocksforfood-wellentriesrevealedamaine ectoflever,F(1,44)=5.35,p=.025,2p=.11,butnomaine ectsofreward,F(1,44)=0.13,p=.720,2p.01,classi cation,F(1,44)=0.00,p=.968,2p.01orblock,F(1,44)=0.35,p=.555,2p.01.Critically,therewasaninteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(1,44)=2.42,p=.127,2p=.05,butnotbetweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,44)=1.33,p=.255,2p=.03,betweenrewardandblock,F(1,44)=0.24,p=.629,2p.01,classi cationandblock,F(1,44)=0.79,p=.380,2p=.02,rewardandlever,F(1,44)=1.35,p=.252,2p=.03,orclassi cationandlever,F(1,44)=0.01,p=.938,2p.01.Therewerenothree-wayinteractionsbetweenreward,classi cationandblock,F(1,44)=0.00,p=.997,2p.01,reward,classi cationandlever,F(1,44)=1.07,p=.307,2p=.02,reward,blockandlever,F(1,44)=1.50,p=.228,2p=.03,orclassi cation,blockandlever,F(1,44)=5.13,p=.028,2p=.10.Therewasnofour-wayinteractionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1,44)=6.71,p=.013,2p=.13.ReversalAcrosstheblocksofreversaltraining(right-handsideoftheupperpanelsofFigure14),leverpressesincreasedforL2anddecreasedduringL1inSTs,howevertheincrease114 waslargerforthe1pelgroup.Lever-pressbehaviourremainednegligibleintheGTgroups.Food-wellbehaviour(right-handsideofthelowerpanelsofFigure14)increasedforL2anddecreasedduringL1.Thedescriptionoftheseresultsissupportedbythefollowinganalysis.LeverpressesANOVAconductedforleverpressesforthereversalstagerevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,

F(1,44)=1.23,p=.273,2p=.03andlever,F(1,44)=2.23,p=.143,2p=.05,butnoe ectofreward,F(1,44)=0.07,p=.795,2p.01,orblock,F(3.48,153.07)=3.29,p=.017,2p=.07.Therewasatwo-wayin-teractionbetweenrewardandlever,F(1,44)=0.43,p=.515,2p.01andbe-tweenblockandlever,F(1.66,72.91)=0.81,p=.427,2p=.02,butnotbe-tweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,44)=0.08,p=.779,2p.01,notbetweenrewardandblock,F(3.48,153.07)=4.53,p=.003,2p=.09,classi cationandblock,F(3.48,153.07)=1.28,p=.282,2p=.03,orbetweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,44)=1.73,p=.195,2p=.04.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenreward,classi cationandlever,F(1,44)=1.49,p=.228,2p=.03andbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(1.66,72.91)=0.49,p=.582,2p=.01,butnotbetweenreward,classi cationandblock,F(3.48,153.07)=2.95,p=.028,2p=.06,orreward,blockandlever,F(1.66,72.91)=0.32,p=.688,2p.01.Therewasnotafour-wayinter-actionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(1.66,72.91)=0.30,p=.697,2p.01.Food-wellentriesANOVAconductedforfood-wellentriesforthereversalstagerevealedmaine ectsofblock,F(3.06,134.81)=0.62,p=.605,2p=.01andlever,F(1,44)=2.21,p=.144,2p=.05,butnotofreward,F(1,44)=2.87,p=.097,2p=.06orclas-si cation,F(1,44)=0.39,p=.536,2p.01.Therewasatwo-wayinteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(2.27,99.98)=5.05,p=.006,2p=.10,butnotbe-tweenrewardandclassi cation,F(1,44)=2.29,p=.137,2p=.05,notbetween115 rewardandblock,F(3.06,134.81)=1.37,p=.253,2p=.03,classi cationandblock,F(3.06,134.81)=0.32,p=.813,2p.01,rewardandlever,F(1,44)=0.11,p=.746,2p.01orbetwee

nclassi cationandlever,F(1,44)=4.27,p=.045,2p=.09.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenreward,classi cationandlever,F(1,44)=6.09,p=.018,2p=.12,butbetweenreward,classi cationandblock,F(3.06,134.81)=0.36,p=.786,2p.01,reward,blockandlever,F(2.27,99.98)=0.83,p=.454,2p=.02,orclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.27,99.98)=0.56,p=.594,2p=.01.Therewasnotafour-wayinteractionbetweenreward,classi cation,blockandlever,F(2.27,99.98)=1.40,p=.250,2p=.03.DiscussionThepurposeofthisexperimentwastoreplicatethe ndingsinExperiment3.How-ever,theresultsfromthisexperimentarenotabletocon rmordisprovetheseresults,asveryfewanimalsshowedsign-trackingbehaviour.ThemediansplitusedtoclassifytheratsinSTandGT,labelsanimalsasGTeveniftheyhaveveryfewlever-presses.Thishasnotbeenanissueinthepast,asthedistributionofSTandGTwasequal,withanimalsthatexclusivelylever-press,othersthatonlyengagewiththefood-well,andamajoritythatdisplaybothtypesofbehaviourdespiteabiastooneorother.Dif-ferencesincohortsinthedistributionofbehaviouraredocumented(e.g.,Fitzpatricketal.,2013),howeverforthisexperimentthedistributionmateriallylimitstheconclusionsthatcanbedrawn.4.1.4DiscussionExperiments3and4ThepurposeofExperiment3andExperiment4wastoinvestigatethepredictionfromHeiDIthatincreasingrewardvalue,andthuspresumablyincreasing US,wouldse-lectivelyincreasetheratesofgoal-trackingbehaviourrelativetosign-trackingbehaviour.Experiment3suggestedthatincreasingUSvaluebyprovidingtworewardpelletsratherthanoneresultsinhigherlevelsofsign-tracking,contrarytoHeiDIpredictions.How-116 ever,Experiment4didnotreplicatethese ndings.Instead,Expe

riment4showednodi erencesinconditionedbehaviourforthemanipulationofrewardmagnitude.TheresultsfromExperiment4,however,needtobetreatedwithcautionbecausethedis-tributionofbehaviour,heavilyskewedtowardsgoal-trackingbehaviour,isprobablynotsensitiveenoughtoprovideanyinsightintothepatternsofleverpressandfood-wellentrybehavioursacrossbothSTandGTanimals.NotwithstandingthepotentialissueswithExperiment4,itisrelativelyclearacrosstheseexperimentsthatincreasingthenumberofrewardpelletsfromonetotwodidnot-asmayhavebeenpredictedbytheHeiDImodel-produceanincreaseingoal-trackingbehaviourrelativetosign-trackingbehaviour.Thatsaid,thesimulationsprovidedinChapter3relatingtodownshiftunblockingdosuggestthatchangingthenumberofrewardsmayhavee ectsbeyondsimplyincreasingtheoverallvalueofreward.ThusamorediagnostictestofthepredictionsdrawnfromHeiDIregardingchangesinrewardmagnitude(andpresumablyofBus)mayinvolvemanipulationsthatdonotconfoundrewardnumberwithmagnitude(e.g.usingdi erenttypesofpelletwhereoneisprefer-abletotheother,ordi erentvolumesofsucrosereward).GiventheimportanceofUSvalueinthecontrolofbehaviour-especiallypotentiallymaladaptivebehaviour(e.g.,Flageletal.,2009;Meyeretal.,2012)-suchfuturestudieswouldbeimportant.Inaddition,whileExperiments3and4didnotconclusivelyaddressoneofthenovelpredictionsfromHeiDI,theydidreinforcethe ndingsfromExperiment1thatreversaloftherewardcontingenciesacrosslevershadafaster/largerimpactongoal-trackingbehaviourthanonsign-trackingbehaviourinbothpredominantlySTorGTanimals.Therelativedi erenceinsensitivitytocontingencyreversalisaccountedforbyHeiDI.4.2Featurepositivee ect4.2.1Introduction

JenkinsandSainsbury(1970)investigateddiscriminationlearninginpigeonsus-117 ingparadigmsinwhichexposedpigeonstoeitherafeaturepositivedesign(FP;e.g.,AB+/B-)orfeaturenegativedesign(FN;e.g.,B+/AB-).Forthefeaturepositivede-sign,acompoundofvisualstimuli(e.g.,AB)wouldbefollowedbyareward,butthepresentationofonlyoneoftheelements(e.g.,B)wouldnotbefollowedbyareward.Forthefeaturenegativedesign(B+,AB-)asingleelementisfollowedbyareward(e.g.,B),butthepresentationofacompoundwhichcontainstheuniqueelementwouldnotbefollowedbyareward(e.g.,AB).TheynoticedthatthebirdsintheFPgroupacquiredthediscriminationrapidlyandceasedrespondingtoBwhenpresentedalone,howeverthebirdsintheFNgroupexhibitedlittleevidenceofsuchlearning.Anotherinteresting ndingwasthatthebirdsstarteddirectingmostofthepecksintheFPgrouptowardsA,theuniquefeature.Thesuperiorityinlearningafeaturepositivediscriminationcom-paredtoafeaturenegativediscriminationisarobuste ectandhasbeenseeninrats(e.g.,Crowell&Bernhardt,1979),honeybees(e.g.,Abramsonetal.,2013),aswellashumans(e.g.,Lotz,Uengoer,Koenig,Pearce,&Lachnit,2012).InthestudyofJenkinsandSainsbury(1970),pigeonsshowednosignoflearninginthefeaturenegativegroup,howeverthereareinstanceswherethisdiscriminationcanbeacquired.Theevidencesuggeststhatlearninginafeaturenegativediscriminationisslowerthaninamatchedfeaturepositivediscrimination,andmayhappenonlyunderparticularconditions(e.g.,morelikelytoobservelearninginasequentialbutnotsimultaneouspresentationofthestimuli,seeGokey&Collins,1980).SimulationsofHeiDI'spredictionsareshowninFigure17andFigure18.Figure17showssimulationsofconditioningforaFPdesign(PanelAandB)andFNdesign(PanelCandD

)forwhenparametersarearrangedforabiastowardsRCS( Aand B=.50, US=.30.).ExaminationsofFigure17(PanelsAandC)showsthatthedi erenceinassociativestrengthbetweenABandBislargerfortheFPthanFNdesign-aclearfeaturepositivee ect.However,whentheseassociativestrengthsaredecomposedintoRCSandRUS(PanelsBandD)itisapparentthatthedi erenceinresponsetendencybetweenABandBbetweenFPandFNdesignsisparticularlyapparentforRCSand118 negligibleforRUS.Thus,HeiDIpredictsthatinanimalsbiasedtosign-tracking,thefeaturepositivee ectshouldbemoreapparentintheirsign-trackingbehaviourthantheirgoal-trackingbehaviour.Figure18showssimulationsofconditioningforaFPdesign(PanelAandB)andFNdesign(PanelCandD)forwhenparametersarearrangedforabiastowardsRUS( Aand B=.30, US=.50).ExaminationofPanelsAandCofFigure18alsoshowsacleardi erenceinassociativestrengthbetweenABandAconsistentwithafeaturepositivee ectwhentheparametersre ectabiastowardsgoal-trackingbehaviour.However,whentheseassociativestrengthsaredecomposedintoRCSandRUS(PanelsBandD)itisapparentthatthedi erenceinresponsetendencybetweenABandBbetweenFPandFNdesignsislargerforRCSthanRUS.Thus,HeiDIpredictsthateveninanimalsbiasedtogoal-tracking,thefeaturepositivee ectshouldbemoreapparentintheirsign-trackingbehaviourthantheirgoal-trackingbehaviour.Totestthesepredictionsratswereexposedtoeitherafeaturepositive(FP)orafea-turenegative(FN)discriminationlearning.Thepurposeofthisexperimentwastoseehowafeaturepositiveandnegativediscriminationlearningtranslatedintoperformance.Moreover,itisknownthatdi erenttypesofstimulileadtodi erentsign-trackingbe-haviour(e.g.,Hearst&F

ranklin,1977).Thesimulationspredictdi erentlevelsofob-serveddi erenceswhenthebiasisarrangedtowardstheCSorUSandforthisreason,forhalfoftheanimals,Aisalever(designTable4)andfortheotherhalfisalight(designTable5),whichpotentiallyimplydi erentlevelsof CS.TheanimalsintheFPgroupwereexposedtotwotypesoftrials:acompoundAB,whichwasfollowedbyareinforcer(AB+)andsinglepresentationsofB,whichwasnon-reinforced(B-).TheanimalsintheFNgroupreceivedpresentationsofthecompoundABwhichwerenotre-inforced(AB-)andpresentationsofB,whichwerefollowedbyareinforcer(B+).BwasalwaysaleverandAwasforhalftheanimalsalever(Experiment5:Levercondition;e.g.,foraparticularanimalAisleftleverandBisrightlever)andfortheotherhalfwasalight(Experiment6:Lightcondition;e.g.,foraparticularanimalAisleftlight119 andBisleftlever).WhenAisalever,Experiment5,IrecordleverpressesandIamabletolookathowtheanimalsinteractbothwithAandB,butforwhenAisalight,theanimalscanapproachandorienttowardsthelight,howeverthisbehaviourwasnotrecorded.4.2.2Experiment5:LeverconditionMethodAnimalsandapparatusThirty-twoListerHoodednavemaleratswereused(suppliedbyCharlesRiver,UK).Theirmeanadlibitumweightbeforethestartoftheexperimentwas286g(range:244-326g)andtheyweremaintainedatbetween85and95%oftheseweightsbygivingthemrestrictedaccesstofoodattheendofeachday.Theratshadcontinuousaccesstowaterwhentheywereintheircages.TheconditioningboxesusedinthisexperimentaredescribedinExperiment1.ProcedureTheratshadtwo24-minpre-trainingsessionswherefoodpelletsweredeliveredonavariable-time(VT)60-sschedule(range:40{80s).Ratsthenreceivedasinglesessionoftrainingoneachofthenext12daysoftraining,whichoccurr

edatthesametimeofdayforagivenrat.Theexperimentconsistedintwoconditions:featurepositive(FP)andfeaturenegative(FN).TheanimalsintheFPgroupreceivedtwotypesoftrials:AB,whichwasfollowedbyareinforcer(AB+)andB,whichwasnon-reinforced(B-).TheanimalsintheFNgroupreceivedpresentationsofABwhichwerenotreinforced(AB-)andpresentationsofB,whichwasfollowedbyareinforcer(B+).AandBweretwolevers(counterbalanced,forhalftheanimalsAwastheleftleverandBtherightlever,andfortheremaininganimalsAwastherightleverandBwastheleftlever).OnABtrials,thetwoleverswerepresentedsimultaneously.Alltheanimalshad20trialsofthecompoundABand20presentationofBpersession,onesessionperday.Theorderinwhichtypeoftrialswerepresentedwasrandomwiththeconstraintthatthere120 couldnotbemorethanthreesametypetrialsinsuccession.Thetrialsweredeliveredonavariable-time(VT)60-sschedule(range:40{80s).Ratswererandomlyassignedtotheexperimentalgroups.Table4:DesignOftheExperiment5:LeverCondition Classi cationGroupTraining STFeaturepositive(FP)AB+/B-Featurenegative(FN)AB-/B+ GTFeaturepositive(FP)AB+/B-Featurenegative(FN)AB-/B+ Note:STreferstoasign-trackerandGTreferstogoal-tracker.Ratswereclassi edasSTorGTonthebasisoftheirbiastowardsleverpressingorenteringthefood-wellduringthe nalblockoftrainingonthereinforcedlever(orcompound).Both\A"and\B"werelevers.\+"denotesthestimulusorcompoundwasreinforced,and\-"denotesthatitwasnon-reinforced.DataAnalysisSuccessivesessionsduringthetrainingwerecombinedinto6Ö2-dayblocks(6Ötraining:T1-T6).Attheendofthetrainingphase,theratsweresplitintotwogroups,sign-trackers(ST)andgoal-trackers(GT),basedontheirtendencytoengagewiththeleverandthefood-well.The

ratsweresplitintotwogroups,STsandGTs,usingthebiasscoredescribedinExperiment1.Biasscoresabove-.12wereclassi edasGTsandbiasscoresbelow-.18asSTs.Thedistributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourisshowninFigure19.MixedANOVAswereconductedseparatelyforleverpressingandfood-wellentrieswithwithin-subjectsfactorsofblock(6levels:T1-T6)andlever(2levels:reinforcedvsnon-reinforced),andbetween-subjectsfactorsfeature(2levels:FPvsFN)andclassi -cation(2levels:STvsGT).Tolookathowratsresponddi erentlytoAandBwhenlearningoccurrsaseparateanalysisisalsopresented,wherecomparisonsaremadeforrespondingtoA,BandB(AB)(whenBispartofthecompoundAB).121 Figure17:Simulationsofconditioningforafeaturepositivedesign(FP;PanelAandB)andafeaturenegativedesign(FN;PanelCandD).PanelsAandCdepicttheoutputvaluesforVCOMB-BandVCOMB-ABandpanelsBandDshowthecorrespondingoutputvaluesforRCSandRUSforbothBandAB.TheparameterswerechosentoresultinabiastowardstheCSwith Aand B=.50, US=.30.ResultsThemainresultsfromExperiment5areshowninFigure20andinFigure21.Myanalysiswillbeginwiththelevercondition,whereIseparatebyresponse(leverpressingandfood-wellentries)andcondition(featurepositiveandfeaturenegative).Thisanalysisalsoincludesseparatingbetween\A",\B"and\B(AB)"forleverpressing.Forthelightcondition,Iseparatebyresponse(leverpressingandfood-wellentries)andcondition(featurepositiveandfeaturenegative).122 Figure18:Simulationsofconditioningforafeaturepositivedesign(FP;PanelAandB)andafeaturenegativedesign(FN;PanelCandD).PanelsAandCdepicttheoutputvaluesforVCOMB-BandVCOMB-ABandpanelsBandDshowthecorrespondingoutputvaluesforRCSandRUSforbothBandAB.Theparameters

werechosentoresultinabiastowardstheUSwith Aand B=.30, US=.50.LeverpressesInspectionoftheleftandright-handsideoftheupperpanelFigure20,whichdepictsleverpressesforthelevercondition,suggeststhattheanimalsintheFPgrouplearnthediscriminationbetweenreinforcedandnon-reinforcedstimulibetterthantheFNgroup.FortheFPconditionbothSTsandGTsshowadiscriminationbetweenreinforcedandnon-reinforcedlever,whichisnotobservedintheFNgroup.Thisdescriptionissupportedbythefollowinganalysis.AnANOVAconductedforleverpressesshowedmaine ectsoffeature(feature123 Figure19:Distributionofsign-tracking(leverpresses)andgoal-tracking(food-wellentries)behaviourforExperiment5per(10-s)trialduringlastblockoftraining(T6)forL1(+jj-).Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).positivevsfeaturenegative),F(1,28)=11.56,p=.002,2p=.29,classi cation,F(1,28)=26.81,p.001,2p=.49,block,F(3.55,99.43)=41.71,p.001,2p=.60,andlever,F(1,28)=68.61,p.001,2p=.71.Therewasalsoaninteractionbe-tweenfeatureandclassi cation,F(1,28)=8.14,p=.008,2p=.23,featureandblock,F(3.55,99.43)=7.05,p.001,2p=.20,classi cationandblock,F(3.55,99.43)=13.83,p.001,2p=.33,criticallybetweenfeatureandlever,F(1,28)=95.95,p.001,2p=.77(re ectingthebetterdiscriminationinthefeaturepositivegroups),classi -cationandlever,F(1,28)=12.62,p=.001,2p=.31andbetweenblockandlever,F(2.15,60.27)=11.87,p.001,2p=.30.Thereweretripleinteractionsbetweenfeature,classi cationandblock,F(3.55,99.43)=5.36,p.001,2p=.16,betweenfeature,classi cationandlever,F(1,28)=10.79,p=.003,2p=.28,andbetween124 Fig

ure20:ResultsfromExperiment5:thee ectsofafeaturepositivedesignonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Mean(+SEM)leverpresses(upperpanels)andfood-wellentries(lowerpanels)per(10-s)trialacrosstraining(T1{T6)separatedbycondition(leftpanels:featurepositive;rightpanels:featurenegative).RatsingroupFPreceivedreinforcedtrialsofacompoundAB\Reinforced(+)"andnon-reinforcedtrialsofB\Non-reinforced(-)"andratsingroupFNreceivednon-reinforcedtrialsofacompoundAB\Non-reinforced(-)"andreinforcedtrialsofB\Reinforced(-)".Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andthegreysymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).feature,blockandlever,F(2.15,60.27)=12.48,p.001,2p=.31,butnotbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.15,60.27)=2.19,p=.117,2p=.07.Thefour-wayinteractionwasnotsigni cant,F(2.15,60.27)=1.65,p=.198,2p=.06.Thcriticalinteractionbetweenfeatureandlever,(F(1,28)=95.95,p.001,2p=.77)isfurtheranalysedtoaddresswhetherthediscriminationisacquiredatallintheFNgroup.Featurepositive:AnANOVAconductedforleverpressesforfeaturepositivede-signshowedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=6.58,p=.022,2p=.32,block,F(1.97,27.61)=11.82,p.001,2p=.46,andlever,F(1,14)=89.35,p.001,2p=.86.Therewereinteractionsbetweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,14)=12.78,125 Figure21:ResultsfromExperiment5splitbytypeofstimuli:AvsBvsB(AB).Mean(+SEM)leverpressestrialacrosstraining(T1{T6)separatedbyclassi cation(leftpanels:ST,sign-trackers;rightpanels:GTs,goal-trackers)anddesign(upperpanels:FP;lowerpanels:FN).RatsingroupFPreceivedreinforcedtrialsofacompoundAB,andnon-reinforcedtrialsofB,andratsingroupFNreceivednon-reinforcedtrialsofacompoundAB,an

dreinforcedtrialsofBp=.003,2p=.48andbetweenblockandlever,F(1.78,24.93)=14.10,p.001,2p=.50,butnotbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(1.97,27.61)=1.62,p=.216,2p=.10,andtherewasnotripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(1.78,24.93)=2.11,p=.147,2p=.13.Next,Ianalysetheinteractionbetweenclas-si cationandleverbyconductingseparatedANOVAsfortheSTandtheGTgroups.AnANOVAconductedfortheSTgroupforleverpresses(FP)showsmaine ectsofblock,F(1.56,9.38)=6.87,p=.018,2p=.53lever,F(1,6)=156.78,p.001,2p=.96,andaninteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(5,30)=8.26,p.001,2p=.58.AparallelanalysisfortheGTgroupshowsmaine ectsofblock,F(5,40)=4.17,p=.004,2p=.34,lever,F(1,8)=14.21,p=.005,2p=.64andaninteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(1.95,15.58)=4.72,p=.026,2p=.37.BothSTandGTlearnthedis-criminationbetweenthereinforcedandthenon-reinforcedlever,howeverthee ectsize126 isbiggerforST.Featurenegative:Aparallelanalysiswasconductedforthefeaturenegativedesignwhichshowedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=20.29,p.001,2p=.59,block,F(5,70)=30.19,p.001,2p=.68,andlever,F(1,14)=6.69,p=.022,2p=.32.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(5,70)=13.28,p.001,2p=.49,butnotbetweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,14)=0.21,p=.654,2p=.01,orcriticallybetweenblockandlever,F(5,70)=0.41,p=.838,2p=.03.Therewasnotatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(5,70)=0.79,p=.562,2p=.05.Itisworthnotingthatthereweremoreresponsesonthenon-reinforcedleverthanthereinforcedlever,theoppositeofanaccuratediscrimination.Food-wellentriesInspectionofthe

leftandright-handsideofthelowerpanelinFigure20,whichdepictsfood-wellentriesforthelevercondition,suggeststhattheGTanimalsintheFPgrouplearnthediscriminationbetweenreinforcedandnon-reinforcedstimulibetterthanST.TheanimalsintheFNgroupdonotlearnthediscrimination.Thisdescriptionissupportedbythefollowinganalysis.AnANOVAconductedforfood-wellentriesrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,28)=31.07,p.001,2p=.53,block,F(2.58,72.32)=3.50,p=.025,2p=.11,andlever,F(1,28)=28.88,p.001,2p=.51,butnoe ectoffeature,F(1,28)=1.62,p=.214,2p=.05.Therewereinteractionsbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.58,72.32)=2.98,p=.044,2p=.10,criticallybetweenfeatureandlever,F(1,28)=18.05,p.001,2p=.39,andbetweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,28)=12.70,p=.001,2p=.31,butnotbetweenfeatureandclassi cation,F(1,28)=1.17,p=.290,2p=.04,orbetweenfeatureandblock,F(2.58,72.32)=1.93,p=.140,2p=.06.Thereweretripleinteractionsbetweenfeature,classi cationandlever,F(1,28)=8.37,p=.007,2p=.23andbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.77,77.65)=7.90,p.001,2p=.22,butnotbetweenfeature,classi cationandblock,F(2.77,77.65)=4.55,p=.007,2p=.14F(2.58,72.32)=0.87,p=.449,2p=.03,127 orbetweenfeature,blockandlever,F(2.77,77.65)=2.50,p=.070,2p=.08.Therewasafour-wayinteraction,F(2.77,77.65)=2.98,p=.040,2p=.10.Featurepositive:AnANOVAconductedforfood-wellentriesinthefeaturepositivedesignshowedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=15.25,p=.002,2p=.52,block,F(2.89,40.52)=4.40,p=.010,2p=.24andlever,F(1,14)=24.15,p.001,2p=.63.Therewereinteractionsbetweenclassi cationan

dblock,F(2.89,40.52)=4.19,p=.012,2p=.23,classi cationandlever,F(1,14)=10.87,p=.005,2p=.44andcriticallyblockandlever,F(2.29,32.04)=4.26,p=.019,2p=.23,andatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(2.29,32.04)=6.42,p=.003,2p=.31.TheresultssuggestthatanimalslearntheFPdiscrimination(blockandleverinteraction,F(2.29,32.04)=4.26,p=.019,2p=.23)howevertheinteractionbetweenclassi cationandlever(F(1,14)=10.87,p=.005,2p=.44)suggestslearningisbetterforGTsanimals.Next,Ianalysetheinteractionbetweenclassi cationandleverbyconductingseparatedANOVAsfortheSTandtheGTgroups.AnANOVAconductedfortheSTgroupforfood-wellentries(FP)showsamaine ectofblock,F(2.38,14.26)=6.50,p=.008,2p=.52andinteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(5,30)=8.31,p.001,2p=.58,howeverthereisnomaine ectoflever,F(1,6)=1.27,p=.303,2p=.17.AparallelanalysisfortheGTgroup,showsmaine ectsofblock,F(5,40)=3.49,p=.010,2p=.30,lever,F(1,8)=36.23,p.001,2p=.82andaninteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(2.04,16.31)=6.09,p=.010,2p=.43.TheseresultssuggestthattheGTgroupshowslearningbetweenthereinforcedandthenon-reinforcedlever.STsshowadi erencebetweenthereinforcedandnon-reinforcedleversuggestedbytheblockandleverinteraction,howeverthisdi erenceislessclearwithnooveralle ectoflever.Featurenegative:Aparallelanalysisconductedforthefeaturenegativedesignre-vealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=16.55,p=.001,2p=.54andlever,F(1,14)=7.63,p=.015,2p=.35,butnotane ectofblock,F(2.12,29.74)=2.26,p=.119,2p=.14.Therewasnotaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.12,29

.74)=1.31,p=.286,2p=.09,classi cationandlever,F(1,14)=2.72,128 p=.121,2p=.16,criticallyblockandlever,F(5,70)=0.75,p=.591,2p=.05,andnotripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(5,70)=1.77,p=.130,2p=.11.Theseresultssuggesttheanimalsmightdiscriminatebetweenthereinforcedandnon-reinforcedstimuli,butthee ectisnotlargeenoughtoshowablockandleverinteraction.NumericallythisseemstobetrueonlyforGTs,possiblyduetoa oore ectinSTs,howevertherewasnointeractionbetweenleverandclassi cation.Analysisbystimulus:AvsBvsB(AB)Aspreviouslymentionedinthedataanalysissection,forExperiment5,bothre-spondingtoAandBwasmeasured,eitheralone,orwithinABcompound.AsthestimuliA,BandB(AB)havedi erentmeaningdependingoftheconditionthiswillbeanalysedseparatedforfeaturepositiveandnegative,respectively.InspectionoftheFPgroup(upperpanelofFigure21),wherethecompoundABwasreinforcedandthepre-sentationofBwasnot,suggeststhatrespondingtothereinforcedcompoundismainlydrivenbyrespondingtoA.Neither,STsorGTslearnthediscriminationbetweenthepresentationofBaspartofthecompound(reinforced)andnon-reinforcedpresentationsofB,interactingmorewithBwhenpresentedalone.InspectionoftheFNgroup(lowerpanelofFigure21),wherethecompoundABwasnon-reinforcedandthepresentationofBwasreinforced,suggeststhattherespondingismainlydrivenbyB,howeverthean-imalsdonotlearnwhenBpredictsornotareinforcer,showingnoevidenceoflearning.Thisdescriptionissupportedbythefollowinganalysis.Featurepositive:AnANOVAconductedforthefeaturepositivedesignonleverpressesintheleverconditionseparatedbystimulus,AvsBvsB(AB),showedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=6.58,p

=.022,2p=.32,block,F(1.97,27.61)=11.82,p.001,2p=.46andstimulus,F(1.07,15.04)=91.27,p.001,2p=.87.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandstimulus,F(1.07,15.04)=11.11,p=.004,2p=.44,andbetweenblockandstimulus,F(2.17,30.31)=16.35,p.001,2p=.54,butnotbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(1.97,27.61)=1.62,p=.216,2p=.10.129 Therewasnotathreewayinteraction,F(2.17,30.31)=1.63,p=.211,2p=.10.Next,IanalyseseparatelyAvsBandBvsB(AB),toseewhichstimulitheanimalsdiscriminatebetween.AnANOVAconductedtheforfeaturepositivedesign,withlevelsforstimulusAvsB,revealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=7.18,p=.018,2p=.34,block,F(1.97,27.61)=14.43,p.001,2p=.51andstimulus,F(1,14)=77.04,p.001,2p=.85.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandstimulus,F(1,14)=12.60,p=.003,2p=.47,andbetweenblockandstimulus,F(2.00,28.06)=15.81,p.001,2p=.53,butnotbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(1.97,27.61)=1.54,p=.233,2p=.10.Therewasnotathree-wayinteraction.F(2.00,28.06)=1.81,p=.182,2p=.11.AparallelcomparisonbetweenBandB(AB),revealedmaine ectsofblock,F(2.73,38.16)=3.64,p=.024,2p=.21,andstimulus(BvsB(AB)),F(1,14)=79.69,p.001,2p=.85,butnotclassi cation,F(1,14)=0.00,p=.991,2p.01.Therewasaninteractionbetweenblockandstimulus,F(5,70)=2.97,p=.017,2p=.18,butnotbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.73,38.16)=0.20,p=.881,2p=.01,orbetweenclassi cationandstimulus,F(1,14)=0.19,p=.673,2p=.01,orathree-wayinteraction,F(5,70)=0.38,p=.858,2p=.03.TheinteractionbetweenblockandstimulusfortheBvsB(AB)analysis(F(5,70)=2.97,p=.017,2p=.18),showsre-spo

ndingtoBbeinghigherwhenpresentedalonethaninthecompound,B(AB).ThisisinconsistentlearningaboutBpartofpartofthecompoundbeingreinforced,suggestingthatalltherespondingfortheABcompoundisdrivenbyrespondingtoA.Featurenegative:Aparallelanalysisforthefeaturenegativedesignonleverpressesintheleverconditionseparatedbystimulus,showedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=20.29,p.001,2p=.59,block,F(5,70)=30.19,p.001,2p=.68andstimulus,F(1.05,14.69)=77.79,p.001,2p=.85.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(5,70)=13.28,p.001,2p=.49,classi cationandstimulus,F(1.05,14.69)=22.26,p.001,2p=.61,blockandstimulus,F(4.02,56.25)=31.61,130 p.001,2p=.69andathree-wayinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandstimu-lus,F(4.02,56.25)=11.75,p.001,2p=.46.AsforFP,next,IanalyseseparatelyAvsBandBvsB(AB),toseebetweenwhichstimulitheanimalsdiscriminate.AnANOVAconductedforfeaturepositivenegative,withlevelsforstimulusAvsB,revealedmaine ectsclassi cation,F(1,14)=17.42,p.001,2p=.55,block,F(2.87,40.14)=23.27,p.001,2p=.62andstimulus(AvsB),F(1,14)=71.23,p.001,2p=.84.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.87,40.14)=12.16,p.001,2p=.46,classi cationandstimulus,F(1,14)=20.51,p.001,2p=.59,andcriticallybetweenblockandstimulus,F(5,70)=33.41,p.001,2p=.70.Therewasatripleinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandstimulus,F(5,70)=13.57,p.001,2p=.49.AparallelcomparisonbetweenBandB(AB),revealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,14)=21.27,p.001,2p=.60,block,F(5,70)=33.02,p.001,2p=.70,butnotstimulus,F(1,14)=0.36,p=.560,2p=.02.There

wasaninteractionbetweenclassi- cationandblock,F(5,70)=13.67,p.001,2p=.49,butnotbetweenclassi cationandstimulus,F(1,14)=0.20,p=.660,2p=.01,blockandstimulus,F(5,70)=1.91,p=.104,2p=.12orathree-wayinteraction,F(5,70)=0.95,p=.454,2p=.06.TheseresultsshowthatevenifthereisnorespondingforAasthenon-reinforcedstimulus(AvsBcomparison,blockandstimulusinteraction,F(5,70)=33.41,p.001,2p=.70),theanimalsdonotlearnaboutthefactthatBisreinforcedwhenpresentedalone(BvsB(AB)analysis,blockandstimulusinteraction,F(5,70)=1.91,p=.104,2p=.12).DiscussionOverall,theseresultsshowafeaturepositivee ect,animalslearnFPdiscrimination,howeverthereislittleevidenceoflearningtheFNdiscrimination.TheanalysissplitbystimulusshowsthatlearningintheFPdesignisdrivenbylearningaboutthefeaturepositive,A.RespondingtoB,doesnotshowlearningaboutB,whenitisornon-reinforced.ItisworthnotingthatbothSTandGTshowlearninginFPaslever131 presses,howeverforfood-wellentriesitisonlyevidentforGTanimals(upperleftpanelofFigure20).ThissupportsHeiDI'spredictionthatafeaturepositivee ectwouldbemoreevidentinCS-orientedresponses.4.2.3Experiment6:LightconditionMethodAnimalsandapparatusThirty-twomaleratswereused(suppliedbyCharlesRiver,UK).Theirmeanadlibitumweightbeforethestartoftheexperimentwas270g(range:220-312g)andtheyweremaintainedatbetween85and95%oftheseweightsbygivingthemrestrictedaccesstofoodattheendofeachday.Theratshadcontinuousaccesstowaterwhentheywereintheircages.TheconditioningboxesusedinthisexperimentaredescribedinExperiment1.ProcedureTheprocedurewasidenticaltoExperiment5withtheexceptionthatBwasalwaysaleverandAwasalightabovethestimulus

B(counterbalanced,forhalftheanimalsBwastheleftleverandAwastheleftlightandfortheotherhalf,BwastherightleverandAwastherightlight).DataanalysisTheanalysishasbeenconductedinasimilarwaytoExperiment5,howeverIdidnotrecordrespondingtothelightinanyway.Thismeansthatthecomparisonbetweenreinforcedandnon-reinforcedleverforthelightconditionisonlybasedonrespondingto\B",theleverwhichwaseitherpresentedaloneorincompoundwiththelight.FortheFPdesign,Bwasreinforcedwhenpresentedwiththelightandnon-reinforcedwhenpresentedaloneandforFNdesign,Bwasreinforcedwhenpresentedaloneandnon-reinforcedwhenpresentedwiththelight.Successivesessionsduringthetrainingwerecombinedinto6Ö2-dayblocks(6Ö132 Table5:DesignOftheExperiment6:LightCondition Classi cationGroupTraining STFeaturepositive(FP)AB+/B-Featurenegative(FN)AB-/B+ GTFeaturepositive(FP)AB+/B-Featurenegative(FN)AB-/B+ Note:STdenotesasign-tracker;GTdenotesagoal-tracker.Ratswereclassi edasSTorGTonthebasisoftheirbiastowardsleverpressingorenteringthefood-wellduringthe nalblockoftrainingonthereinforcedlever(orcompound).\A"wasalightand\B"wasalever.\+"denotesthestimulusorcompoundwerereinforced,and\-"denotestheywerenon-reinforced.training:T1-T6).Attheendofthetrainingphase,theratsweresplitintotwogroups,sign-trackers(ST)andgoal-trackers(GT),basedontheirtendencytoengagewiththeleverandthefood-well,asdescribedinExperiment1.Biasscoresabove.08wereclassi edasGTsandbiasscoresbelow-.08asSTs.Thedistributionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingbehaviourisshowninFigure22.ResultsLeverpressesInspectionoftheleftandright-handsideoftheupperpanelinFigure23,whichdepictsleverpressesforthelightcondition,suggeststha

tneithertheanimalsintheFPgrouporintheFNgrouplearnthediscriminationbetweenreinforcedandnon-reinforcedstimuli.InbothFPandFNdesigns,STsshowhighernumberofleverpressesthanGTs.Thisdescriptionissupportedbythefollowinganalysis.AnANOVAconductedforthelightconditiononleverpressesrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,28)=11.24,p=.002,2p=.29,andblock,F(2.14,60.02)=8.55,p.001,2p=.23,butnoe ectoffeature,F(1,28)=0.04,p=.838,2p.01orlever,F(1,28)=0.27,p=.607,2p.01.Therewerenointeractionsbetweenfeatureandclassi cation,F(1,28)=0.18,p=.678,2p.01,orbetweenfeatureandblock,133 Figure22:Distributionofsign-tracking(leverpresses)andgoal-tracking(food-wellentries)behaviourforExperiment6per(10-s)trialduringlastblockoftraining(T6)forL1(+jj-).Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andtheclearsymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).F(2.14,60.02)=0.20,p=.833,2p.01,classi cationandblock,F(2.14,60.02)=3.03,p=.052,2p=.10,criticallyfeatureandlever,F(1,28)=1.27,p=.269,2p=.04,classi cationandlever,F(1,28)=0.59,p=.449,2p=.02orbetweenblockandlever,F(5,140)=1.39,p=.231,2p=.05.Therewerenotripleinteractionsbe-tweenfeature,classi cationandblock,F(2.14,60.02)=0.17,p=.855,2p.01,betweenfeature,classi cationandlever,F(1,28)=0.52,p=.477,2p=.02,fea-ture,blockandlever,F(5,140)=2.02,p=.080,2p=.07orbetweenclassi cation,block,lever,F(5,140)=1.03,p=.403,2p=.04.Therewasnofour-wayinteraction,F(5,140)=0.51,p=.772,2p=.02.Astherewasnointeractionbetweenfeatureandlever(F(1,28)=1.27,p=.269,2p=.04)nofurtheranalysisarepresentedforleverpresses.Theseresultssuggestthattheani

malsdonotshowlearningineithertheFP134 Figure23:ResultsfromExperiment6:thee ectsofafeaturepositivedesignonsign-trackingandgoal-tracking.Mean(+SEM)leverpresses(upperpanels)andfood-wellentries(lowerpanels)per(10-s)trialacrosstraining(T1{T6)separatedbycondition(leftpanels:featurepositive;rightpanels:featurenegative).RatsingroupFPreceivedreinforcedtrialsofacompoundAB\Reinforced(+)"andnon-reinforcedtrialsofB\Non-reinforced(-)"andratsingroupFNreceivednon-reinforcedtrialsofacompoundAB\Non-reinforced(-)"andreinforcedtrialsofB\Reinforced(-)".Theblacksymbolscorrespondtosign-trackers(ST)andthegreysymbolstogoal-trackers(GT).orFNdesign.Food-wellentriesInspectionoftheleftandright-handsideofthelowerpanelinFigure23,whichdepictsfood-wellentriesforthelightcondition,suggeststhattheGTsanimalsintheFPgrouplearnthediscriminationbetweenreinforcedandnon-reinforcedstimuli.TheanimalsintheFNgroupshownoevidenceoflearning.Thisdescriptionissupportedbythefollowinganalysis.ANOVAconductedforthelightconditiononfood-wellentriesrevealedmaine ectsofclassi cation,F(1,28)=18.88,p.001,2p=.40,andlever,F(1,28)=17.86,p.001,2p=.39,butnomaine ectsoffeature,F(1,28)=0.02,p=.881,2p.01135 orblock,F(3.09,86.63)=0.65,p=.593,2p=.02.Therewereinteractionsbetweenfeatureandlever,F(1,28)=7.99,p=.009,2p=.22,betweenclassi cationandlever,F(1,28)=7.70,p=.010,2p=.22andbetweenblockandlever,F(3.47,97.22)=5.30,p=.001,2p=.16,butnotbetweenfeatureandclassi cation,F(1,28)=0.02,p=.881,2p.01,featureandblock,F(3.09,86.63)=0.55,p=.653,2p=.02,orbetweenclassi -cationandblock,F(3.09,86.63)=2.14,p=.099,2p=.07.Ther

eweretripleinteractionsbetweenfeature,blockandlever,F(3.47,97.22)=3.95,p=.008,2p=.12andbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(3.47,97.22)=2.28,p=.075,2p=.08,notbetweenfeature,classi cationandblock,F(3.09,86.63)=0.49,p=.698,2p=.02,orbetweenfeature,classi cationandlever,F(1,28)=5.32,p=.029,2p=.16.Therewasafour-wayinteractionbetweenfeature,classi cation,blockandlever,F(3.47,97.22)=3.05,p=.026,2p=.10.Featurepositive:AnANOVAconductedforfood-wellentriesforthefeaturepos-itivedesignrevealedmaine ectsoflever,F(1,14)=13.02,p=.003,2p=.48,butnoe ectofclassi cation,F(1,14)=7.81,p=.014,2p=.36orblock,F(2.32,32.48)=0.17,p=.871,2p=.01.Therewasaninteractionbetweenclassi- cationandlever,F(1,14)=6.76,p=.021,2p=.33,andcriticallybetweenblockandlever,F(3.07,43.04)=5.46,p=.003,2p=.28,butnotbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.32,32.48)=1.01,p=.386,2p=.07.Therewasathree-wayinteractionbetweenclassi cation,blockandlever,F(3.07,43.04)=3.07,p=.036,2p=.18.Next,IconductseparateanalysisfortheSTandGTgroups,toseewhichanimalshavelearntthediscriminationbetweenthereinforcedandnon-reinforcedstimulus.AnANOVAconductedfortheSTgroupshowsamaine ectoflever,F(1,4)=4.02,p=.115,2p=.50,butnoe ectofblock,F(5,20)=1.30,p=.303,2p=.25andnointeractionbetweenblockandlever,F(2.46,9.85)=1.14,p=.370,2p=.22.AparallelanalysisfortheGTgroup,showsamaine ectoflever,F(1,10)=22.61,p.001,2p=.69andaninteractionbetweenblockandlever,F(5,50)=9.89,p.001,2p=.50,butnoblocke ect,F(2.27,22.70)=0.96,p=.408,2p=.09.TheseresultssuggestthatGTsshow136 bett

erdiscriminationbetweenthereinforcedandnon-reinforcedstimulusthanSTs.Featurenegative:Aparallelanalysisconductedforthefeaturenegativeconditionre-vealedamaine ectofclassi cation,F(1,14)=12.45,p=.003,2p=.47,butnotlever,F(1,14)=11.00,p=.005,2p=.44,orane ectofblock,F(2.12,29.74)=1.40,p=.262,2p=.09.Therewerenointeractionsbetweenclassi cationandblock,F(2.12,29.74)=1.89,p=.166,2p=.12,classi cationandlever,F(1,14)=1.23,p=.286,2p=.08,critciallybetweenblockandlever,F(3.68,51.50)=0.30,p=.864,2p=.02,orathree-wayinteraction,F(3.68,51.50)=0.58,p=.663,2p=.04.TheseresultssuggeststhattheanimalsdonotlearntheFNdiscrimination.DiscussionTheseresultsshowsomeevidenceofFPdiscriminationlearning,mainlydrivenbytheGTsfood-wellentries,butagainnoevidenceoflearningtheFNdiscrimination.Experiment5showedthatthefeaturepositivee ectismainlyexplainedbytheanimals'respondingtotheuniquefeature,whichinthelightconditionisnotmeasured.4.2.4DiscussionExperiments4and5TheresultsfromExperiment5and6showoverallthattheanimalslearnafea-turepositivebetterthanafeaturenegativediscrimination,wheretheyshowedlittleevidenceoflearning.Therewasadi erencebetweentheleverandlightconditions,astheanimalsshowedlearningthediscriminationbymainlyinteractingwithA(Figure21),andinthelightcondition(Experiment6)behaviourtowardsAwasnotmeasured.DirectingbehaviourtowardstheuniquefeatureisconsistentwiththeresultsfoundbyJenkinsandSainsbury(1970).Sign-trackersexpressedlearningmoreinleverpressesandgoal-trackersinfood-wellentries,whichwasmoreobviousintheExperiment5(Leverexperiment,(Figure23).InExperiment6,leverpressesshowedlittlelear

ningintheFPdesign,howeverthisisprobablyduetothefactthatbehaviourtowardsAwasnotmeasured(Figure23).Goal-trackingratsshowlearningofafeaturepositive137 discriminationinfood-wellentries.Thishighlightstheimportanceofhowlearningisassessed,asdi erentmeasurementscouldleadtodi erentconclusions.Forexample,inExperiment6(Lightcondition),onlymeasuringbehaviourtowardsBwouldleadustoconcludeeitherthatSTshavenotlearntafeaturepositivediscriminationorthatlearningismainlyshowedbyinteractingwiththefood-well.Experiment5(Levercondition)revealsthefactthatlearningisobservedinbothleverpressesandfood-wellentriesinbothSTsandGTs,howeveritismainlydrivenbytheuniquefeature.HeiDIpredictedafeaturepositivee ect,howeveritpredictedthatwhileaFNdis-criminationishardertolearnbutitstillshouldbelearnt.OthernovelpredictionsderivedfromHeiDIarediculttoassessastheyrelyontheanimalslearningafeaturenegativediscrimination,howeverthegeneralpredictionthatitshouldbemoreobviousinleverpressesthanfood-wellentrieswasnotpresentintheoverallresultsbecausesuchane ectwasapparentinExperiment5butnotExperiment6(albeit,sign-trackingororientationtothepositivefeaturelightwasnotassessed).Mostassociativelearningmodelspredictafasteracquisitionofafeaturepositivediscrimination,howevertheyalsofailtopredictnolearninginafeaturenegativediscrimination(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;McLaren,Kaye,&Mackintosh,1989;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner,1981).TheconditionsunderwhichtheanimalsareabletolearnaFNdiscriminationarestilltobeelucidated.138 Chapter5GeneralDiscussionTheoriesofassociativelearningassumethatpairingsofaCSwithaUSresultintheformationofanassociationbetweenthece

ntralrepresentationsoftheevents(e.g.,Mackintosh,1975;Pearce&Hall,1980;Rescorla&Wagner,1972;Wagner,1981).ThisassociationallowstheCStoactivatetherepresentationorideaoftheUSandtherebybehaviour.Thesetheoriesassumeamonotonicrelationshipbetweenasingleconstructthatrepresentslearningandacquiredbehaviour.Thisviewischallengedbythefactthatasimpleauto-shapingprocedureproducesmarkedindividualdi erencesinbehaviour:someratspredominantlyinteractwiththeCS(e.g.,lever),othersinvestigatethelocationwheretheUSisabouttobedelivered,andtheremaindershowpatternsofbehaviourinbetweenthesetwoextremes(e.g.,Patituccietal.,2016).Focusingononemeasure(e.g.,leverpresses)leadstotheconclusionthatonegroup(sign-trackers)betteracquiretheCS-USassociation,whilefocusingonanothermeasure(e.g.,food-wellentries)leadstotheoppositeconclusion.Thepurposeofthisthesiswastoincreaseourunderstandingofhowlearningistranslatedintoperformance,andtheoriginofindividualdi erencesinhowlearningisexpressed.TheempiricalresultsfromChapter2contributedtothedevelopmentofanewmodelwhichintegrateslearningandperformance,HeiDI(Chapter3).Thismodelprovidesnewapproachtoknownphenomena,whichinthepasthaveposedachallengeformanylearningtheories,butitalsoprovidesnewtestablepredictionsabouthowlearningistranslatedinconditionedbehaviour.InChapter4Itestedsomeofthesenovelpredictions.Iwill rstsummarisethenovelempirical ndings,fromChapter2and4,beforemovingontodiscusstheimplicationsofthetheoreticaladvancesprovidedbyHeiDI.139 5.1Empirical ndingsThefewattemptsatexplainingindividualdi erencesinconditionedbehaviourhaveappealedtotheassumptionoftwolearningsystems(e.g.,S-S/S-R,model-based

/model-free),eachofthemgeneratingonetypeofbehaviour(e.g.,Lesaintetal.,2014).Thereareatleasttwoformsthatthisanalysiscouldtake.Onepossibilityisthatthetwolearningsystemsoperatedi erentlyacrossrats,thebalancefavouringonesystemoranother.Ifasinglesystemgovernedallbehaviourinagivenratthenbothfood-wellandlever-pressresponsesshouldexhibitthecharacteristicpropertyofthatsystem:WhengovernedbyanS-Ssystem(ormodelbased),activitydirectedtowardboththeleverandthefood-wellwillchangerapidlyinthefaceofachangeincontingencies;whereaswhengovernedbyanS-R(model-free)systembothwillchangerelativelyslowly.Anotherpossibilityisthatbothsystemsoperateinparallelinagivenrat,howevereachsystemgeneratesonetypeofbehaviour.Thisanalysispredictsthatagivenformofresponsewillexhibitthesamecharacteristicsindependentlyofwhethertheanimalinwhichitisobservedisclassi edasaSToraGT;withfood-wellactivitybeingderivedfromtheoperationofaS-Ssystem(ormodelbased)andlever-orientedbehaviourbeingderivedfromaS-Rsystem(ormodel-free)thatoperatetodi erentdegreesinallrodents.Thedominantresponsemightbetowardthefood-wellinonerodentandleverinanother,butinbothratsfood-wellactivityshouldmorerapidlytrackchangesinreinforcementcontingenciesthanshouldlever-orientedactivity.ThesepredictionswereassessedinExperiment1andExperiment2.InExperiment1ratsreceiveddiscriminationtrainingwherethepresentationofonelever(L1)waspairedwithfoodpelletsandanotherlever(L2)wasnon-reinforced.Whenratslearntthediscrimination,thecontingenciesontheleverswerereversedwithL1nownon-reinforcedandL2reinforced.Thisprocedureresultedinmarkedindividualdi erencesinconditionedresponding;withsomeratsinteractingwit

hL1(butnotL2)andothersapproachingthesiteoffooddeliveryduringL1(butnotL2).Ratswereclassi edasST140 andGTonthebasisoftheirrespondinginthelastblockoftraining.Resultsshowedthatwhenthecontingencieswerereversed,thedi erentlevelsofleverpressingtoL1(andL2)inGroupsSTandGTremainedremarkablystableduringthe rstblockofreversal.Incontrast,thelevelsoffood-wellentrieschangedmorerapidlyinbothGroupsSTandGT(Figure2).InExperiment2ratswerepresentedwithtwolevers(L1andL2),bothreinforcedwiththesametypeofreinforcer.AsnotedinChapter2,thereweretwogroupsofanimalswhichhadtwodi erentreinforcers:onegrouphadpelletsasareinforcerandanothergrouphadsucrose.Previousworkinmylabsuggestedpelletssustainmorerespondingthansucrose.Afterinitialtraining,thecontingenciesontheleverswerethenswitched.Thegroupofanimalsthathadpelletsasareinforcerreceivedsucroseand,thegroupthathadsucrosereceivedpellets.Whenthecontingencieswereswitched,bothSTandGT,changedtheirrespondingforfood-wellentries,increasingtheirrespondingfromsucrosetopelletsanddecreasingtheirrespondingfrompelletstosucrose.Lever-pressrespondingremainedstablefollowingtheswitchincontingencies(Figure5).InbothExperiment1andExperiment2,lever-pressbehaviourwaslesssensitivetochangesinreinforcementcontingenciesthanwasfood-wellbehaviour.Thisdi erenceinsensitivitywasequallyapparentinratsthatwereclassi edasSTsandGTs.Tosummarize,theseresultsindicatethatdi erentialsensitivitytochangeisapropertyofthebehaviourthatisbeingmeasurednotanoverallpropertyoftheanimalbeinggovernedbyonesystemoranother.AsdiscussedinChapter3(section3.6.1.Excitatoryconditioningandextinction),HeiDIpredictsthatVCS�USandVCOM

Bdecline,butVUS�CSdoesnot,whichleadstodi erentialsensitivitytoUSchanges:CS-orientatedbehaviourisextinguishedslowerthanUS-orientedbehaviour.TheresultsobtainedinExperiment1andExperiment2arepredictedbyHeiDI.Experiments3,4,5and6aimedtotestnewpredictionderivedfromHeiDI.HeiDIassumesthat CSand USare xedforagivenCSandUSinagivenanimal,141 butproposethattheperceivedsalienceoftheCS(relatingto CS)andUS(relatingto US),andhencec* CSandVCS�US(Equations4and5),canvarybetweenanimals.Experiment3examinedhowperformanceisa ectedbyasimplemanipulation,thenumberoffoodpellets(oneortwo),whichconstitutedtheUSonagiventrial.HeiDIpredictshigherlevelsofUS-orientedbehaviour(orgoal-tracking)forUSsofhighervalue.However,theresultsfromthisexperiment,showedthataUSassumedtohaveahighervalue(2pellets)producedmoresign-trackingcomparedtoaUSwithalowervalue(1pellet),andnodi erenceswereobservedingoal-tracking.ThesampleofratsinExperiment3wasrelativelylow,andExperiment4aimedtoreplicatethe ndings.However,thedistributionofbehaviourinthiscohort,withveryfewanimalsthatshowedanysign-trackingbehaviour,madetheinterpretationoftheresultsinExperiment4dicult.Thisexperimentcouldnotbeexpectedtoreplicatethe ndingsofExperiment3,astheanimalsbehavedatypically.Discriminationlearningparadigmsshowthatpigeonsacquiremorerapidlyafea-turepositivediscrimination(FP;e.g.,AB+/B-)thanafeaturenegativediscrimination(FN;e.g.,B+/AB-)(Jenkins&Sainsbury,1970).Thefeaturepositivee ectisawell-establishedcharacteristicofPavlovianphenomenagenerally,butrelativelylittleresearchhasinvestigatedhowittranslatesintoconditionedbehaviour.HeiDIpredicts

afeaturepositivee ect:learningshouldbebetterinanFPdesignthananFNdesign.WhenthebiasisarrangedtowardstheCS(when CS� US,Figure17),thisismoreevidentinRCSthanRUS.WhenthebiasisarrangedtowardtheUS(when CS US,Figure18),foraFPdesignthisdiscriminationshouldbemoreevidentinRCSthanRUS,howeverforanFNdesignthediscriminationisequalorbetterforRUScomparedtoRCS(again,suggestingthefeaturepositivee ectshouldbemoreapparentinRCSthanRUS).Simulationspredicteddi erentlevelsofobserveddi erenceswhenthebiasisarrangedtowardstheCSorUSandforthisreasonforhalfoftheanimalsAisalever(Experiment5)andfortheotherhalfisalight(Experiment6),whichpotentiallyimplydi erentlevelsof .142 TheresultsfromExperiment5andExperiment6,showoverallthattheanimalslearntthefeaturepositivediscrimination,howeverforthefeaturenegativediscriminationtheyshowedlittleevidenceoflearning.Interestingly,thefeaturepositivee ectwasmoreobviousinExperiment5,whererespondingtoA(theuniquefeature)wasmeasured.DirectingbehaviourtowardstheuniquefeatureisconsistentwiththeresultsfoundbyJenkinsandSainsbury(1970).Itisunclearwhytheanimalsdidnotlearnthefeaturenegativediscriminationbutitispossible,eventhoughunlikely,theamountoftrainingwasnotenough.AnotherpossibilityliesintheuseofsimultaneousrathersuccessivepresentationoftheABcompound.OthernovelpredictionsderivedfromHeiDI,suchasthefeaturepositivee ectbeingmoreevidentinsign-trackingthangoaltracking,arediculttoassess.Thesepredictionsrelyonthefactthatanimalsacquireafeaturenegativediscrimination.Theseexperimentshighlight,yetagain,theimportanceofhowlearningisassessed,asdi erentmeasurementscouldl

eadtodi erentconclusions.IftheconclusionweremadeonthebasisofExperiment6(Lightcondition),measuringbehaviourtowardsBwouldleadtotwowrongconclusions.First,IwouldbeinclinedtoconcludethatSTsdonotlearnafeaturepositivediscriminationorsecond,thatlearningismainlyexpressedbyinteractingwiththefood-well.Experiment5(Levercondition)revealsthefactthatlearningisobservedinbothleverpressesandfood-wellentriesinbothSTsandGTs,howeveritismainlydrivenbyrespondingtotheuniquefeature,whichinExperiment6isnotmeasured.Overalltheempiricalworkpresentedinthisthesiselucidatesoneimportantquestion:isittheanimalorthebehaviour?Someofthepreviousresultshavebeenattributedtotheanimals'\personality"(e.g.,Ahrensetal.,2016;Bissonetteetal.,2015;Flageletal.,2009;Lesaintetal.,2014;Lovicetal.,2011).However,theresultspresentedhereindicatethattheapparent\in exibility"or\resistancetochange"isapropertyofthebehaviourbeingmeasured.Anopenremainingquestionconcernswhetherauni edsinglemechanismtheory(e.g.,HeiDI)isabletoexplainhowlearningtranslatesinbehaviour,oradual-mechanismisnecessarytoexplainalltheobserveddi erencesin143 conditionedbehaviour.Theempiricalworkpresentedherehasnotfullyaddressedthiscrucialquestion.TheresultsobtainedinExperiment1andExperiment2areconsistentwithbothoftheseaccounts.Experiment3andExperiment4couldhaveprovidedevidencefortheoriginofin-dividualdi erencesandiftheyarerelatedtotheUSvalue,howevertheresultswereinconclusive.OthernovelpredictionsaboutthedistributionofconditionedbehaviourbetweentheCSandtheUS,wereaddressedinExperiment5andExperiment6,howevertheyreliedontheanimalslearningafeaturenegativediscrimination.InthefollowingsectionIwil

ldiscussbothhowHeiDIisabletoaccommodateknownphenomena,whichhaveposedachallengeinthepasttootherlearningtheories(in-cludingthosethatrelyondual-processmechanismstoexplainthepattersofsign-andgoal-trackingbehaviour),anddescribeothernovelpredictionsofhowlearningistrans-latedintoperformance.5.2Theoreticalimplications:HeiDIIndispellingout-dated(academictextbook)descriptionsofPavlovianconditioning,Rescorla(1988,p.151)referredtothreeprimaryissuestobeaddressedinthestudyofanylearningprocess:\Whatarethecircumstancesthatproducelearning?Whatisthecontentofthelearning?Howdoesthatlearninga ecttheorganism'sbehaviour?".ItisperhapsespeciallysurprisingthatinthecontextofPavlovianlearningthe nalissue{concerningconditionedbehaviouritself-hasbecomesecondarytotheorizingdirectedtowardaddressingthe rsttwoquestions.IndeedformaltheoriesofPavlovianlearninghaveoftenfollowedthesimplifyingstanceexpressedbyRescorlaandWagner(1972)thatitis\sucientsimplytoassumethatthemappingofVsintomagnitudeorprobabilityofconditionedrespondingpreservestheirordering.".ThefactthattheformofconditionedbehaviourdependsonthenatureofboththeCSandUS(e.g.,Holland,1977,1984)andthattherearemarkedindividualdi erencesinhowlearningisexhibited144 (e.g.,Patituccietal.,2016)representasigni cantimpetusfordevelopingtheoriesthatrecognizethisvariety.HeiDIdoesthis.5.2.1Conditions,contentandperformanceIstartedbysimplifyingtheRescorla-Wagnerlearningruleforforward,CS-USas-sociations,andsupplementingitwithaformallyequivalentruleforreciprocal,US-CSassociations(seeEquation1and2).Thevaluesreturnedbytheseequationswerethencombined(toformVCOMB)usingarulethatweightstheassocia

tivevalueofthestim-ulusthatispresent(e.g.,VCS�US)morethananassociationinvolvingassociativelyactivatednodes(e.g.,VUS�CS;seeEquation3).Finally,whentheCSispresented,VCOMBisdistributedintwoformsofbehaviour(CS-oriented,RCS,andUS-oriented,RUS)accordingtotheratioof CSandVCS�US(seeEquation4and6).Theresultingmodel,HeiDI,providesthefollowinganswerstothethreequestionsposedbyRescorla(1984):(1)Onagiventrial,learningoccurstotheextentthatthereisadi erencebe-tweentheperceivedsalienceofanevent(re ectedin US)andtheperceivedsalienceoftheretrievedrepresentationofthateventbasedonthecombinedassociativestrengthsofthestimulipresentedonthattrial(PVTOTAL�US;oradi erencebetweenc* CSandPVTOTAL�CS).(2)Learningisrepresentedinthereciprocalassociationsbetweenthenodesactivatedbydi erentstimuli(e.g.,CSandUS).(3)Performancere ectstherelativeintensityoftheCS(asre ectedin CS)andtheassociativestrengthoftheCS(VCS�US;whichre ects USthroughc* US)multipliedbythecombinedassocia-tivestrengthsinvolvingtheCSandUS(i.e.,VCOMB).Inthisway,HeiDIprovidesawaytocapturetwoclassesofconditionedbehaviour,andindividualdi erencestherein,togetherwiththee ectofgroup-levelmanipulations.IhavehighlightedtheapplicationofHeiDItosign-trackingandgoal-tracking,whichareexamplesofthegeneraldistinctionbetweenCS-orientedandUS-orientedbehaviours.Thespatialseparationofthetwoclassesofresponseandtheeasewithwhichtheyareautomaticallyrecordedcertainlymeansthattheyhavesomemethodologicaladvan-145 tagesoverotherresponses(e.g.,thoseelicitedbyaversiveUSs).Nevertheless,IassumethatmanyPavlovianconditioningproceduresresultingreatervarietyi

nconditionedresponsesthanisroutinelymeasuredandusedtoguidetheorizing.Ihavealreadyil-lustratedhowthispracticemightcomplicateinterpretationofpatternsofresultsinthecaseofblocking.However,thetwoclassesofresponsesthatIhaveconsideredmightthemselvesbefurtherdivided,withtheindividualelementsoftheCSandUSgivingrisetothedi erentresponsesde ned(r1-6;seeJenkins&Moore,1973).ExpandingHeiDItoaccommodatethiscomplexitywouldnotpresentspeci ctheoreticalchallenge:witheachindividualelementhavingitsown or valuesandaliated(unconditioned)re-sponses.However,thereareissuesthatdorequirefurtherdiscussion.Theseinvolvehowassociationsbetweenthecomponentsofacompoundstimulusmighta ectperformance,andthenatureoftherepresentationsoftheCSandUS.5.2.2AssociationsbetweenthecomponentsofacompoundConditionedrespondingtoaCSisnotonlydeterminedbywhetherithasadirectassociationwithaUS.Forexample,afterexposuretoastimuluscompound(AB),condi-tionedrespondingthatisestablishedtoBwillalsobeevidentwhenAispresented(e.g.,Rescorla&Cunningham,1978).Thise ectisknownassensorypreconditioninganditisoftenattributedtotheformationofanassociativechainthatallowsAtoactivatetheUSthroughA-BandB-USassociations(butsee,Lin&Honey,2016).IhavealreadyprovidedananalysisofhowA-Blinksmightform(seeEquation7and8),andhaveappealedtosuchlinksinprovidingananalysisofdownshiftunblocking(cf.Rescorla&Colwill,1983).ThewayinwhichthelinksinthechaincanbecombinedtodeterminethelevelofperformancegeneratedbyAcanbederivedfromanextensionofEquation3:VCHAIN=1/c*VA�BVCOMB�B,whereVCOMB�B=VB�US+(1/c*VB�US*VUS�B).ThisformulationmeansthatVCHAINVCOMB�BifVA�B1.Theway

inwhichVCHAINisdistributedintoRCSandRUScanbedeterminedusingEquations4and5:c* Aissubstitutedforc* CS,j1/c*VA�B*VB�USjissubstitutedforj1/c*VCS�USj,146 andVCHAINreplacesVCOMB.IntermsofthenatureofthebehaviourelicitedbyA,themostobviouspredictionisthatitwillmirrorthatevokedbyBthroughdirectcondition-ing(Holland,1984).However,accordingtoHeiDIthedistributionofCS-orientedandUS-orientedbehaviourwilldi erbetweenAandB:withCS-orientedrespondingbeingmoreevident(andUS-orientedbehaviourlessevident)duringAthanduringB:Totheextentthatwhilec* Aandc* Bwillbethesame,j1/c*VA�B*VB�USjjVB�USj(seeDwyer,2012).Thisanalysisofsensorypreconditioning,andofthepotentialimpactofwithin-compoundassociationsinconditioningproceduresmorebroadly,isrelativelystraightforward.However,thereisanotherapproachtoconditionedperformancethathasalsobeenappliedtosensorypreconditioningandcuecompetitione ects(e.g.,over-shadowingandblocking).ItdeservesconsiderationbecauseitaddressessomeofthesameissuesandphenomenaasHeiDI.ThecomparatormodelproposedbyStoutandMiller(2007)focusesonhowperfor-mancetoateststimulus,A,isa ectedbythestimuliwithwhichitwastrained(e.g.,BafterconditioningwithanABcompound).Thismodelbuildsontheideathatper-formancetoAattestisdeterminedbyacomparisonbetween(i)therepresentationoftheUSdirectlyretrievedbyA,and(ii)therepresentationofthesameUSindirectlyretrievedbytheassociativechain:A-BandB-US(seeMiller&Matute,1998).Inthiscase,BiscalledthecomparatorstimulusforA,andfollowingpairingsofABwithaUS,thetendencyforAtogenerateperformanceattestisheldtoberestrictedbythefactthatitscomparator,B,hasretrievedamemoryoftheUS.Theanal

ysistherebyexplainsovershadowingandblocking,butalsoother ndingsthatareproblematicforanunreconstructedRescorla-Wagnermodel.However,inthecaseofsensoryprecondi-tioning,whereABis rstnon-reinforced,themodelisforcedtoassumethatthefactthatBhasacquiredexcitatoryassociativepropertiesduringasecondstageincreasesthepotentialforAtogenerateperformance.Thesedi eringe ectsofthecomparatorterm(B;termedsubtractiveandadditive)areheldtobedeterminedbyexperiencewithcomparingtheUSrepresentationretrievedbyAwiththeUSrepresentationindirectly147 retrievedbyB.Theadditivee ectoccurswhentherehasbeenlittleornoopportunitytoexperiencethetwotypesofretrievedrepresentations(e.g.,duringsimpleexposuretoABinsensorypreconditioning),andthesubtractivee ectincreaseswithexperiencethata ordssuchacomparison(e.g.,duringmulti-trialcompoundconditioning;Stout&Miller,2007,p.765).Inanycase,liketheRescorla-Wagnermodel,themoreso-phisticatedanalysisofperformancedevelopedbyStoutandMiller(2007)providesnoreadyexplanationforthefactthatdi erentbehaviouralmeasurescanprovidesupportforopposingconclusionsabouthowassociativestrengthistranslatedintoperformance,whichisthefocusofinteresthere.Thatbeingsaid,thefactthatwithinHeiDIthedistri-butionofCS-orientedandUS-orientedcomponentsofperformancere ectstherelativevaluesofc* andVCS�USinvolvesacomparisonprocessofsorts.Certainly,changingtheassociativestrengthofstimulibeforetestingwillnotonlya ectVCOMB,butwillalsoa ectRCSandRUSthroughchangingVCS�US.AsIhavealreadynoted,inthecontextofmypreviousdiscussionofblocking,asecureinterpretationoftheimpactofsuchchangesonperformancerequiresbehaviourala

ssaysthataresensitivetobothRCSandRUS.5.2.3Elementalandcon guralprocessesA nalissue,whichImentionedinthesectiononsummation(section3.6.4),con-cernshowmodelsthatdonothavecon guralprocessesaddressthefactthatanimalscanlearndiscriminationsthatarenotlinearlyseparable.Forexample,animalscanlearnthatatonesignalsfoodandaclickersignalsnofoodinoneexperimentalcontextandthetonesignalsnofoodandaclickersignalsfoodinasecondcontext(seeAllmanetal.,2004).Thistypeofdiscriminationisinterestingbecausean`elemental'animal{oneonlycapableofrepresentingindividualevents{shouldbeincapableoflearningthem:Thetoneandclickerhavethesamereinforcementhistory,asdothespottedandcheckedchambers,andthereforeeachofthefourcombinationsorcompoundsshouldbeequallycapableofgeneratingperformance.Thereisanongoingdebateabouthow148 di erentcombinationsofthesamestimulimightberepresentedinwaysthatwouldpermitthesediscriminationstobeacquired(e.g.,Brandonetal.,2000;Honeyetal.,2010;Pearce,1994).Forexample,di erentstimuluselementsofagivenauditorystim-ulusmightbecomeactivedependingonthecontextinwhichtheyareencountered(e.g.,Brandonetal.,2000),ortheelementsactivatedbyagivenpatternofstimulationmightcometoactivateasharedcon guralrepresentation(e.g.,Honeyetal.,2010;Pearce,1994).Ineithercase,theelementsorcon gurationsthereof(orboth;seeHoney,Ior-danova,&Good,2014)couldbesubjecttothesamelearningandperformancerulesdescribedinEquations1to6(seealsoDelamater,2012).However,Ishouldalsonotethattheresponseunits(r1-r6)withintheproposedassociativearchitectureforHeiDI(seeFigure7provideanotherlocusinwhichcombinationsofCSsandindeedUSsmightberepresented:Thestrengthofth

econnectionsfromcombinationsofCSsandUSstotheseresponseunitscouldbemodi edduringconditioning(forarelateddiscussion,seeHoneyetal.,2010).5.2.4LimitationsandfurtherdevelopmentIhavealreadynotedthatEquation6providesasimplisticanalysisofhowchangesinRCSandRUSmighta ectactivityinasetofresponse-generatingunits(r1-r6).However,takingastepback,whatisneededinordertoprovideadetailedassessmentoftheaccuracyofthepredictionsthatIhavederivedfromHeiDI,isestimatesoftheperceivedsalienceofboththeCSandUSonanindividual-by-individualbasis.Armedwiththeseestimates,Icouldthenprovideaquantitativeanalysisofthe tbetweenpredictionsofthemodelandthebehaviourofanimalsonanindividualbasis.IthasbeenarguedthatpalatabilitymightprovideanestimateofperceivedUSsalience(cf.Patituccietal.,2016),andonepotentialestimateoftheperceivedsalienceofaCSistheunconditionedorientingbehaviourthatitspresentationprovokesbeforeconditioninghastakenplace(cf.Kaye&Pearce,1984).149 5.2.5ConcludingcommentsPavlovianconditioninghasprovidedafertiletestbedinwhichtoinvestigateissuesconcerningwhenassociativelearningoccurs,itscontent,andhowitistranslatedintoperformance.Ofthesethreeissues,formalmodelshavepaidleastattentiontohowlearn-ingistranslatedintoperformance:considerationofperformancehasbeensecondarytoanalysesoftheconditionsandcontentoflearning.HeiDIbeginstoredressthisim-balancebyprovidinganintegratedanalysisofallthreeissues.Thisanalysiscouldbedevelopedinordertoprovideamorequantitativeanalysis,modellingperformanceatanindividual-by-individuallevel,withthecharacteristicsoftheschematicnetworkfullyspeci ed.Asalreadynoted,itcouldalsobeextendedtoexplicitlydistinguishbetweend

i erentfeaturesofboththeCSandtheUS,whichcouldbetiedtodi erenttypesofresponse(seealsoDelamater,2012).Intheprocessofdevelopingthisrelativelysimplemodel,ithasbecomeclearthatitisdiculttoaddressoneofRescorla'sthreeissueswithoutadetailedconsiderationoftheothers:developingamorecompleteunderstand-ingofassociativelearningthroughthestudyofPavlovianconditioninginvolvesmultipleconstraintsatisfaction(Marr,1982).HeiDIprovidesgeneralinsightsintolearning,itscontentandperformancethatare{atleastinpart{bornoutofamoredetailedanalysisofthevarietyandindividualdi erencesinconditionedbehaviour.Thisevidencehasbeentoooftenneglected,givenitstheoreticalimportanceandpotentialtranslationalsigni cance.ItistruethattheempiricalresultspresentedherearenotfullyconsistentwiththenovelpredictionsofHeiDI:increasingthenumberofreinforcersdidnotselectivelyin u-encegoal-trackingoversigntrackinginExperiment3andExperiment4;andthefeaturepositivee ectwasnotgenerallylargerforsign-thangoal-trackingresponses(compareExperiment5withExperiment6).However,therearereasonstothinkthatthesemayhavebeenduetoissuesotherratherthan awedpredictions:changingreinforcernumbermaynotbeadirectin uenceon US;andtheabsenceoffeaturenegativediscriminationmakesassessingthefeaturepositivee ectambiguous.So,bothdualprocess(e.g.,S-Svs150 S-R;ormodelfreevsmodelbased)accountsofthedi erencesinsign-andgoal-trackingremainasviableasthesingle-processaccounto eredbyHeiDI.Thatsaid,thesedualprocessaccountsdonoto erthesamerangeofnovelexplanationsforpriorresultsthathaveprovedproblematicforexistingassociativetheory.Thus,thereisaclearargumenttobemadeforthesuperio

rityofthetypeofsingle-processaccounto eredbyHeiDI.151 Chapter6Appendix6.1ReproducibilityandOpenScienceFramework(OSF)ThisdocumentisSweaveRproject.ItcombinesSweave les( .Rnw extension)whenRcodeneedstobeintegratedwithinthetextorsimpleLATEX les( .tex extension)whenonlytextisneeded.ThedataandthecodearefreelyavailableonOSF(https://osf.io/h4fyg)website.TheREADME le(OSF/Thesis/README/README.pdf)detailsthestructureoftheSweaveRproject.ForreproducibilitypurposesthecodefordatacleaningandmanipulationofthedatafromalltheexperimentsreportedherehavebeenuploadedontheOpenScienceFramework/Experiments(osf.io/h4fyg)website.Thecodesfortheexperimentsarethefollowing:ˆExperiment1:DDR066(Experiments/2017)ˆExperiment2:RH001(Experiments/2017)ˆExperiment3:DDR074(Experiments/2017)ˆExperiment4:DDR084(Experiments/2018)ˆExperiment5and6:DDR087(Experiments/2018)6.2HeiDIappForconductingthesimulations,IbuiltaShinydashboardapp(HeiDI)usingRprogramminglanguage.ThecodeforthesimulationstogetherwithaREADME le152 whichdetailsthenecessarystepstoreproducethesimulationsisavailableontheOSFwebsite(https://osf.io/h4fyg/).Appstructureand lesThemain leis`app.R'.This leisresponsibleforthemainbodyoftheappandsourcesthe lesneededtoconductthesimulations:`formulas.html',`load.packages',`model.R',`my theme.R',`style.css'aswellasallthe lesinthe`phenomenon'folder.app.R:Wheneverpossible,tocreateacleanerappearancetheappsourcesother les.The ui functionbuildsauserinterfaceandthe server de nestheserverlogic.Morein-formationaboutshinyappscanbefoundathttps://shiny.rstudio.com/tutorial/written-tutorial/lesson1

/.formulas.html:writestheformulasdisplayedinthe'Formulas'tabinhtmllanguage.install packages.R:this leinstallsallthepackagesusedforthisapp.load packages.R:sourcesfromtheapptoloadallthepackages.model.R:containsfunctionsformodelling,convertingfromwidetolongformatandlabellingdataframes.Each'model'functionhasacomplementary`to.long'(transformsgenerateddatatolongformat)and`label'(re-labelsvariablestouseforplots)function.Every`model'functionhascommentedthephenomenonforwhichitisbeingused.Therearethreefunctionsandeachofthemisusedforasetnumberofvariables(e.g.two valuesandone value).my theme.R:containsfunctionsforcontrollingplotformatting(e.g.textsize,spac-ing,axisticks)in`ggplot'plots.phenomenonfoldercontains lessourcedfromtheapptocreatesimulationsforeachofthephenomenon.READMEfolder:containsthe/README.tex lethatwasusedtogeneratethisdocument.rsconnectfolder:generatedbytheapp.wwwfolder:/style.css:controlsappearance(e.g.textformat,boxsizes,colour)oftheapp.153 logfolder:containslog lestotrackchangesintheapp.Eachnumbered log letrackschangesusingatimestamp.Theappisnot nalandwillincludesimulationsofseveralmorephenomena.6.3AdditionalnotesForreferencegenerationthe\natbib"package(Daly&Ogawa,2013)hasbeenused.AspecialthankyoufortheStackover owcommunityforallthesupportinallR,SweaveandLATEXrelatedquestions.Chapter4,Experiment5andExperiment6:Intheappconditionedinhibitionsimu-latesonlyFNdesignAB-/B+(FPnotsimulatedyetintheapp).Thishasnoimplicationfortheconclusionsdrawn,howeveritisworthmentioningforreproducibilitypurposes.154 ReferencesAbramson,C.I.,Cakmak,I.,Duell,M.E.,Bates-Albers,L.M.,Zuniga,E.M.,Pende-gra

ft,L.,...Wells,H.(2013).Feature-positiveandfeature-negativelearninginhoneybees.TheJournalofexperimentalbiology,216(Pt2),224{9.Ahrens,A.M.,Singer,B.F.,Fitzpatrick,C.J.,Morrow,J.D.,&Robinson,T.E.(2016).Ratsthatsign-trackareresistanttoPavlovianbutnotinstrumentalextinction.Behaviouralbrainresearch,296,418{430.Aitken,M.R.F.,Bennett,C.H.,McLaren,I.P.L.,&Mackintosh,N.J.(1996).Perceptualdi erentiationduringcategorizationlearningbypigeons.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,22(1),43{50.Allman,M.J.,&Honey,R.C.(2005).Associativechangeinconnectionistnetworks:anaddendum.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,31(3),363{7.Allman,M.J.,Ward-Robinson,J.,&Honey,R.C.(2004).Associativechangeintherepresentationsacquiredduringconditionaldiscriminations:furtheranalysisofthenatureofconditionallearning.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,30(2),118{28.Anselme,P.,Robinson,M.J.F.,&Berridge,K.C.(2013).Rewarduncertaintyenhancesincentivesalienceattributionassign-tracking.BehaviouralBrainResearch,238,53{61.Arcediano,F.,Escobar,M.,&Miller,R.R.(2005).BidirectionalAssociationsinHu-mansandRats.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,31(3),301{318.Asch,S.E.,&Ebenholtz,S.M.(1962).Theprincipleofassociativesymmetry.Pro-ceedingsoftheAmericanPhilosophicalSociety,106,135{163.Atkinson,R.C.,&Estes,W.K.(1963).Stimulussamplingtheory.InR.D.Luce,R.R.Bush,&E.Galanter(Eds.),Handbookofmathematicalpsychology(Vol2ed.).NewYork:Wiley.155 Auguie,B.,&Anotonov,A.(2017).package`gridExtra':MiscellaneousFunctionsfor"Grid"Graphics.Retrievedfromhttps://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/gridExtra/index.h

tmlBaker,A.G.,&Mercier,P.(1982).Priorexperiencewiththeconditioningevents:Evidenceforarichcognitiverepresentation.InM.L.Commons,R.J.Hernstein,&A.R.Wagner(Eds.),Quantitativeanalysesofbehavior(pp.117{143).Cambridge,MA:Ballinger.Barnet,R.C.,&Miller,R.R.(1996).Second-orderexcitationmediatedbyaback-wardconditionedinhibitor.Journalofexperimentalpsychology.Animalbehaviorprocesses,22(3),279{96.Beierholm,U.R.,Anen,C.,Quartz,S.,&Bossaerts,P.(2011).Separateencodingofmodel-basedandmodel-freevaluationsinthehumanbrain.NeuroImage,58(3),955{962.Bissonette,G.B.,Orsini,C.A.,Simon,N.W.,Calu,D.J.,Nasser,H.M.,Chen,Y.,&Fiscella,K.(2015).Individualvariabilityinbehavioral exibilitypredictssign-trackingtendency.FrontBehavNeurosci,9,Article289.Blanchard,R.J.,Fukunaga,K.K.,&Blanchard,C.(1976).Environmentalcontrolofdefensivereactionstofootshock(Vol.8;Tech.Rep.No.2).Boughner,R.L.,&Papini,M.R.(2003).Appetitivelatentinhibitioninrats:Nowyouseeit(signtracking),nowyoudon't(goaltracking).AnimalLearning&Behavior,31(4),387{392.Bouton,M.E.(1984).Di erentialcontrolbycontextinthein ationandreinstate-mentparadigms.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,10(1),56{74.Bouton,M.E.(2004).ContextandBehavioralProcessesinExtinction.Learning&Memory,11(5),485{494.Brandon,S.E.,Vogel,E.H.,&Wagner,A.R.(2000).Acomponentialviewofcon guralcuesingeneralizationanddiscriminationinPavlovianconditioning.Behavioural156 BrainResearch,110(1-2),67{72.Brimer,C.J.(1970).Inhibitionanddisinhibitionofanoperantresponseasafunctionoftheamountandtypeofpriortraining.PsychonomicScience,21(3),191{192.Brimer,C.J.,&Kamin,L.J.(1963).Disinhibiton,habituation,sensitizatio

n,andtheconditionedemotionalresponse.JournalofComparativeandPhysiologicalPsychology,56(3),508{516.Brown,P.L.,&Jenkins,H.M.(1968).Auto-shapingofthepigeon'skey-peck.JournaloftheExperimentalAnalysisofBehavior,11(1),1{8.Browne,M.P.(1976).Theroleofprimaryreinforcementandovertmovementsinautoshapinginthepigeon(Vol.4;Tech.Rep.No.3).Bush,R.R.,&Mosteller,F.(1951).Amodelforstimulusgeneralizationanddiscrimi-nation.PsychologicalReview,58(6),413{423.Cinotti,F.,Marchand,A.R.,Roesch,M.R.,Girard,B.,&Khamassi,M.(2019).Impactsofinter-trialintervaldurationonacomputationalmodelofsign-trackingvs.goal-trackingbehaviour.Psychopharmacology,2373{2388.Cohen-Hatton,S.R.,Haddon,J.E.,George,D.N.,&Honey,R.C.(2013).Pavlovian-to-instrumentaltransfer:Paradoxicale ectsofthePavlovianrelationshipexplained.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,39(1),14{23.Cole,R.P.,&Miller,R.R.(1999).ConditionedExcitationandConditionedInhibitionAcquiredthroughBackwardConditioning.LearningandMotivation,30(2),129{156.Costa,D.S.,&Boakes,R.A.(2009).Contextblockinginratautoshaping:Sign-trackingversusgoal-tracking.LearningandMotivation,40(2),178{185.Cotton,M.M.,Goodall,G.,&Mackintosh,N.J.(1982).Inhibitoryconditioningresultingfromareductioninthemagnitudeofreinforcement.TheQuarterlyjournalofexperimentalpsychology.B,Comparativeandphysiologicalpsychology,34(Pt3),163{80.Crowell,C.R.,&Bernhardt,T.P.(1979).Thefeature-positivee ectandsign-tracking157 behaviorduringdiscriminationlearningintherat.AnimalLearning&Behavior,7(3),313{317.Daly,P.W.,&Ogawa,A.(2013).package`natbib':Flexiblebibliographysupport.Retrievedfromhttps://ctan.org/pkg/natbib?lang=enDavey,G.

C.L.,&Cleland,G.G.(1982).Thee ectofpartialreinforcementontheacquisitionandextinctionofsign-trackingandgoal-trackingintherat(Vol.19;Tech.Rep.No.2).Daw,N.D.,Gershman,S.J.,Seymour,B.,Dayan,P.,&Dolan,R.J.(2011).Model-basedin uencesonhumans'choicesandstriatalpredictionerrors.Neuron,69(6),1204{15.Dayan,P.,&Berridge,K.C.(2014).Model-basedandmodel-freePavlovianrewardlearning:revaluation,revision,andrevelation.Cognitive,a ective&behavioralneuroscience,14(2),473{92.Delamater,A.R.(2012).OnthenatureofCSandUSrepresentationsinPavlovianlearning.Learning&Behavior,40(1),1{23.Derman,R.C.,Schneider,K.,Juarez,S.,&Delamater,A.R.(2018).Sign-trackingisanexpectancy-mediatedbehaviorthatreliesonpredictionerrormechanisms.Learning&Memory,25(10),550{563.Dickinson,A.,&Balleine,B.W.(2002a).Theroleoflearninginmotivation.InC.Gallistel(Ed.),Stevens'handbookofexperimentalpsychology.vol.3.(pp.497{533).Wiley.Dickinson,A.,&Balleine,B.W.(2002b).TheRoleofLearningintheOperationofMotivationalSystems.InStevens'handbookofexperimentalpsychology.Hoboken,NJ,USA:JohnWiley&Sons,Inc.Dickinson,A.,Hall,G.,&Mackintosh,N.J.(1976).Surpriseandtheattenuationofblocking.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,2(4),313{322.Duvarci,S.,Nader,K.,&LeDoux,J.E.(2008).DenovomRNAsynthesisisrequiredfor158 bothconsolidationandreconsolidationoffearmemoriesintheamygdala.Learning&Memory,15(10),747{755.Dwyer,D.M.(2012).LickingandLiking:TheAssessmentofHedonicResponsesinRodents.QuarterlyJournalofExperimentalPsychology,65(3),371{394.Dwyer,D.M.,Burgess,K.V.,&Honey,R.C.(2012).Avoidancebutnotaversionfol-lowingsensorypreconditioningwith avors:Achallengetostimulussubsti

tution.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,38(4),359{368.Dwyer,D.M.,Figueroa,J.,Gasalla,P.,&Lopez,M.(2018).RewardAdaptationandtheMechanismsofLearning:ContrastChangesRewardValueinRatsandDrivesLearning.PsychologicalScience,29(2),219{227.Dwyer,D.M.,&Mackintosh,N.J.(2002).Alternatingexposuretotwocompound a-vorscreatesinhibitoryassociationsbetweentheiruniquefeatures.Animallearning&behavior,30(3),201{7.Engberg,L.A.,Hansen,G.,Welker,R.L.,&Thomas,D.R.(1972).AcquisitionofKey-PeckingviaAutoshapingasaFunctionofPriorExperience:"LearnedLaziness"?Science,178(4064),1002{1004.Fitzpatrick,C.J.,Gopalakrishnan,S.,Cogan,E.S.,Yager,L.M.,Meyer,P.J.,Lovic,V.,...Morrow,J.D.(2013).VariationintheFormofPavlovianConditionedAp-proachBehavioramongOutbredMaleSprague-DawleyRatsfromDi erentVen-dorsandColonies:Sign-Trackingvs.Goal-Tracking.PLoSONE,8(10),e75042.Flagel,S.B.,Akil,H.,&Robinson,T.E.(2009).Individualdi erencesintheat-tributionofincentivesaliencetoreward-relatedcues:Implicationsforaddiction.Neuropharmacology,56(SUPPL.1),139{148.Flagel,S.B.,Clark,J.J.,Robinson,T.E.,Mayo,L.,Czuj,A.,Willuhn,I.,...Akil,H.(2011).Aselectiverolefordopamineinstimulus{rewardlearning.Nature,469(7328),53{57.Flanagan,B.,&Webb,W.B.(1964).Disinhibitionandexternalinhibitionin xedintervaloperantconditioning.PsychonomicScience,1(1-12),123{124.159 Gallistel,C.R.(1990).Theorganizationoflearning.Cambridge,MA:MITPress.Gallistel,C.R.,&Gibbon,J.(2000).Time,RateandConditioning.PsychologicalReview,107(2),289{344.Gluck,M.A.,&Bower,G.H.(1988).Fromconditioningtocategorylearning:Anadaptivenetworkmodel.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:General,117(

3),227{247.Gokey,D.S.,&Collins,R.L.(1980).Conditionedinhibitioninfeaturenegativediscriminationlearningwithpigeons.AnimalLearning&Behavior,8(2),231{236.Gottlieb,D.A.(2004).Acquisitionwithpartialandcontinuousreinforcementinpigeonautoshaping.Learning&behavior,32(3),321{34.Gottlieb,D.A.(2006).E ectsofpartialreinforcementandtimebetweenreinforcedtrialsonterminalresponserateinpigeonautoshaping.BehaviouralProcesses,72(1),6{13.Gromer,D.(2019).package`apa':FormatOutputsofStatisticalTestsAccordingtoAPAGuidelines.Retrievedfromhttps://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/apa/index.htmlGrossberg,S.(1980).Howdoesabrainbuildacognitivecode?PsychologicalReview,87,1{51.Hall,G.(1991).PerceptualandAssociativeLearning.Oxford,UK:OxfordUniversityPress.Hall,G.,&Honey,R.C.(1989).Contextuale ectsinconditioning,latentinhibition,andhabituation:Associativeandretrievalfunctionsofcontextualcues.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,15(3),232{241.Hall,G.,&Minor,H.(1984).ASearchforContext-StimulusAssociationsinLatentInhibition.TheQuarterlyJournalofExperimentalPsychologySectionB,36(2b),145{169.Hearst,E.,&Franklin,S.R.(1977).Positiveandnegativerelationsbetweenasignal160 andfood:Approach-withdrawalbehaviortothesignal.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,3(1),37{52.Hearst,E.,&Jenkins,H.M.(1974).Signtracking:Thestimulus-reinforcerrelationanddirectedaction.Austin:TX:PsychonomicSociety.Hinrichs,J.V.(1968).Disinhibitionofdelayin xed-intervalinstrumentalconditioning.PsychonomicScience,12(7),313{314.Holland,P.C.(1977).ConditionedstimulusasadeterminantoftheformofthePavlovianconditionedresponse.JournalofExperimenta

lPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,3(1),77{104.Holland,P.C.(1984).OriginsofBehaviorinPavlovianConditioning.PsychologyofLearningandMotivation,18,129{174.Holland,P.C.(1988).Excitationandinhibitioninunblocking.Journalofexperimentalpsychology.Animalbehaviorprocesses,14(3),261{79.Holmes,N.M.,Chan,Y.Y.,&Westbrook,R.F.(2019).Acombinationofcommonandindividualerrortermsisnotneededtoexplainassociativechangeswhencueswithdi erenttraininghistoriesareconditionedincompound:AreviewofRescorla'scompoundtestprocedure.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalLearningandCognition,45(2),242{256.Honey,R.C.,&Bolhuis,J.J.(1997).Imprinting,Conditioning,andWithin-eventLearning:.Quantitativeanalysesofbehavior,50(2),97{110.Honey,R.C.,&Good,M.(1993).Selectivehippocampallesionsabolishthecontextualspeci cityoflatentinhibitionandconditioning.Behavioralneuroscience,107(1),23{33.Honey,R.C.,Iordanova,M.D.,&Good,M.(2010).Latentinhibitionandhabituation:evaluationofanassociativeanalysis.InR.E.Lubow&I.Weiner(Eds.),Latentinhibition(pp.163{182).Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Honey,R.C.,Iordanova,M.D.,&Good,M.(2014).Associativestructuresinan-imallearning:Dissociatingelementalandcon guralprocesses.Neurobiologyof161 LearningandMemory,108,96{103.Honey,R.C.,&Ward-Robinson,J.(2002).Acquiredequivalenceanddistinctivenesofcues:I.Exploringaneuralnetworkapproach.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,28(4),378{387.Hull,C.L.(1943).Principlesofbehavior.NewYork:Appleton-Century-Crofts.Hull,C.L.(1949).Stimulusintensitydynamism(V)andstimulusgeneralization.PsychologicalReview,56(2),67{76.JASPTeam.(2018).JASP[Computersoftware].Jenkins,H.M.,&

Moore,B.R.(1973).Theformoftheauto-shapedresponsewithfoodorwaterreinforcers.Journaloftheexperimentalanalysisofbehavior,20(2),163{81.Jenkins,H.M.,&Sainsbury,R.S.(1970).Discriminationlearningwiththedistinctivefeatureonpositiveandnegativetrials.InD.Mostofsky(Ed.),Attention:Contem-porarytheoryandanalysis(pp.239{273).NewYork:Appleton-Century-Crofts.Kamin,L.J.(1969).Selectiveassociationandconditioning.InN.J.Mackintosh&W.K.Honig(Eds.),Fundamentalissuesinassociativelearning(pp.42{89).Halifax:DalhousieUniversityPress.Kaye,H.,&Pearce,J.M.(1984).ThestrengthoftheorientingresponseduringPavlo-vianconditioning.Journalofexperimentalpsychology.Animalbehaviorprocesses,10(1),90{109.Konorski,J.(1948).Conditionedre exesandneuronorganization.Cambridge:Cam-bridgeUniversityPress.Konorski,J.(1967).Integrativeactivityofthebrain.Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.Kruschke,J.K.(1992).ALCOVE:anexemplar-basedconnectionistmodelofcategorylearning.Psychologicalreview,99(1),22{44.Lawrence,M.A.(2016).package`ez':EasyAnalysisandVisualizationofFactorialExperiments.Retrievedfromhttps://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ez/162 index.htmlLee,B.,Gentry,R.N.,Bissonette,G.B.,Herman,R.J.,Mallon,J.J.,Bryden,D.W.,...Roesch,M.R.(2018).Manipulatingtherevisionofrewardvalueduringtheintertrialintervalincreasessigntrackinganddopaminerelease.PLOSBiology,16(9),e2004015.Lesaint,F.,Sigaud,O.,Flagel,S.B.,Robinson,T.E.,&Khamassi,M.(2014).ModellingIndividualDi erencesintheFormofPavlovianConditionedApproachResponses:ADualLearningSystemsApproachwithFactoredRepresentations.PLoSComputBiol,10(2).Leslie,J.C.,&Millenson,J.R.(1996).Principlesofbehavioralanalysis.Amsterdam,TheNeth

erlands:HarwoodAcademicPublishers.Lin,T.E.,&Honey,R.C.(2016).LearningAboutStimuliThatArePresentandThoseThatAreNot.InThewileyhandbookonthecognitiveneuroscienceoflearning(pp.69{85).Chichester,UK:JohnWiley&Sons,Ltd.Lonsdorf,T.B.,Weike,A.I.,Nikamo,P.,Schalling,M.,Hamm,A.O.,&Ohman,A.(2009).GeneticGatingofHumanFearLearningandExtinction.PsychologicalScience,20(2),198{206.Lotz,A.,Uengoer,M.,Koenig,S.,Pearce,J.M.,&Lachnit,H.(2012).Anexplorationofthefeature-positivee ectinadulthumans.Learning&Behavior,40(2),222{230.Lovic,V.,Saunders,B.T.,Yager,L.M.,&Robinson,T.E.(2011).Ratspronetoattributeincentivesaliencetorewardcuesarealsopronetoimpulsiveaction.BehaviouralBrainResearch,223(2),255{261.Lubow,R.E.(1989).Latentinhibitionandconditionedattentiontheory.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Lubow,R.E.,&Moore,A.U.(1959).Latentinhibition:Thee ectofnonreinforcedpre-exposuretotheconditionalstimulus.JournalofComparativeandPhysiologicalPsychology,52(4),415{419.Mackintosh,N.J.(1974).Thepsychologyofanimallearning.London:AcademicPress.163 Mackintosh,N.J.(1975).Atheoryofattention:Variationsintheassociabilityofstimuliwithreinforcement.PsychologicalReview,82(4),276{298.Mackintosh,N.J.(1976).Overshadowingandstimulusintensity.AnimalLearning&Behavior,4(2),186{192.Maes,E.,Boddez,Y.,Alfei,J.M.,Krypotos,A.M.,D'Hooge,R.,DeHouwer,J.,&Beckers,T.(2016).Theelusivenatureoftheblockinge ect:15failurestoreplicate.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:General,145(9),e49{e71.Marr,D.(1982).Vision:Acomputationalinvestigationintothehumanrepresentationandprocessingofvisualinformation.Cambridge,MA:MITPress.Matzel,L.D.,Han,Y.R.,Grossman,H.,Karnik,M.S.,Patel,D

.,Scott,N.,...Gandhi,C.C.(2003).Individualdi erencesintheexpressionofa\general"learningabilityinmice.TheJournalofneuroscience:theocialjournaloftheSocietyforNeuroscience,23(16),6423{33.Matzel,L.D.,&Sauce,B.(2017).Individualdi erences:Casestudiesofrodentandprimateintelligence.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalLearningandCognition,43(4),325{340.McClelland,J.L.,&Rumelhart,D.E.(1981).Aninteractiveactivationmodelofcontexte ectsinletterperception:I.Anaccountofbasic ndings.PsychologicalReview,88(5),375{407.McLaren,I.P.L.,Kaye,H.,&Mackintosh,N.J.(1989).Anassociativetheoryoftherepresentationofstimuli:applicationstoperceptuallearningandlatentinhibition.InR.Morris(Ed.),Paralleldistributedprocessing:Implicationsforpsychologyandneurobiology(pp.102{130).Oxford:ClarendonPress.McLaren,I.P.L.,&Mackintosh,N.J.(2000).Anelementalmodelofassociativelearn-ing:I.Latentinhibitionandperceptuallearning.AnimalLearning&Behavior,28(3),211{246.Meyer,P.J.,Lovic,V.,Saunders,B.T.,Yager,L.M.,Flagel,S.B.,Morrow,J.D.,&Robinson,T.E.(2012).Quantifyingindividualvariationinthepropensityto164 attributeincentivesaliencetorewardcues.PLoSONE,7(6),e38987.Miller,R.R.,Barnet,R.C.,&Grahame,N.J.(1995).AssessmentoftheRescorla-Wagnermodel.Psychologicalbulletin,117(3),363{86.Miller,R.R.,&Matute,H.(1998).CompetitionBetweenOutcomes.PsychologicalScience,9(2),146{149.Miller,R.R.,&Matzel,L.D.(1988).TheComparatorHypothesis:AResponseRuleforTheExpressionofAssociations.PsychologyofLearningandMotivation,22,51{92.Morrison,S.E.,Bamkole,M.A.,&Nicola,S.M.(2015).Signtracking,butnotgoaltracking,isresistanttooutcomedevaluation.FrontiersinNeuroscience,9(DEC),

1{12.Mundy,M.E.,Dwyer,D.M.,&Honey,R.C.(2006).Inhibitoryassociationscontributetoperceptuallearninginhumans.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,32(2),178{184.Murphy,R.A.,&Honey,R.C.(2016).TheWileyHandbookontheCognitiveNeuro-scienceofLearning(R.A.Murphy&R.C.Honey,Eds.).Chichester,UK:JohnWiley&Sons,Ltd.Ostlund,S.B.,&Balleine,B.W.(2008).Onhabitsandaddiction:Anassociativeanalysisofcompulsivedrugseeking.Drugdiscoverytoday.Diseasemodels,5(4),235{245.Patitucci,E.,Nelson,A.J.D.,Dwyer,D.M.,&Honey,R.C.(2016).TheOriginsofIndividualDi erencesinHowLearningIsExpressedinRats:AGeneral-ProcessPerspective.JExpPsycholAnimLearnCogn,42(4),313{324.Pavlov,I.P.(1927).Conditionedre exes.London:OxfordUniversityPress.Pavlov,I.P.(1930).Abriefoutlineofthehighernervousactivity.InC.Murchison(Ed.),Psychologiesofthe1930(pp.207{220).Worcester,MA:ClarkUniversitiesPress.Pavlov,I.P.(1941).Conditionedre exesandpsychiatry.NewYork,NY:International165 Publishers.Pearce,J.M.(1994).Similarityanddiscrimination:aselectivereviewandaconnec-tionistmodel.Psychologicalreview,101(4),587{607.Pearce,J.M.,Aydin,A.,&Redhead,E.S.(1997).Con guralanalysisofsummationinautoshaping.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,23(1),84{94.Pearce,J.M.,&Hall,G.(1980).AmodelforPavlovianlearning:Variationsinthee ectivenessofconditionedbutnotofunconditionedstimuli.PsychologicalReview,87(6),532{552.Pearce,J.M.,&Mackintosh,N.J.(2010).Twotheoriesofattention:Areviewandpossibleintegration.InC.Mitchel&M.E.LePelley(Eds.),Attentionandasso-ciativelearning:Frombraintobehaviour(pp.11{39).Oxford:OxfordUniversityPress.Pearce,J.M.,Redhead,E.S.,&George,

D.N.(2002).Summationinautoshapingisa ectedbythesimilarityofthevisualstimulitothestimulationtheyreplace.Journalofexperimentalpsychology.Animalbehaviorprocesses,28(2),175{89.Pena-Oliver,Y.,Giuliano,C.,Economidou,D.,Goodlett,C.R.,Robbins,T.W.,Dalley,J.W.,&Everitt,B.J.(2015).Alcohol-PreferringRatsShowGoalOrientedBehaviourtoFoodIncentivesbutAreNeitherSign-TrackersNorImpulsive.PLoSONE,10(5),e0131016.Rescorla,R.A.(1968).ProbabilityofshockinthepresenceandabsenceofCSinfearconditioning.Journalofcomparativeandphysiologicalpsychology,66(1),1{5.Rescorla,R.A.(1969).Pavlovianconditionedinhibition.PsychologicalBulletin,72(2),77{94.Rescorla,R.A.(1971).Variationinthee ectivenessofreinforcementandnonrein-forcementfollowingpriorinhibitoryconditioning.LearningandMotivation,2(2),113{123.Rescorla,R.A.(1973).E ectofUShabituationfollowingconditioning.Journalof166 comparativeandphysiologicalpsychology,82(1),137{43.Rescorla,R.A.(1988).Pavlovianconditioning.It'snotwhatyouthinkitis.TheAmericanpsychologist,43(3),151{60.Rescorla,R.A.(2000).Associativechangesinexcitorsandinhibitorsdi erwhentheyareconditionedincompound.Journalofexperimentalpsychology.Animalbehaviorprocesses,26(4),428{38.Rescorla,R.A.(2001).Unequalassociativechangeswhenexcitorsandneutralstimuliareconditionedincompound.TheQuarterlyjournalofexperimentalpsychology.B,Comparativeandphysiologicalpsychology,54(1),53{68.Rescorla,R.A.,&Colwill,R.M.(1983).Within-compoundassociationsinunblocking.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,9(4),390{400.Rescorla,R.A.,&Cunningham,C.L.(1978).Within-compound avorassociations.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehav

iorProcesses,4(3),267{275.Rescorla,R.A.,&Wagner,A.R.(1972).Atheoryofpavlovianconditioning:vari-ationsinthee ectivenessofreinforcementandnonreinforcement.InB.A.H.&W.Prokasy(Eds.),Clasicalconditioningii:currentresearchandtheory(pp.64{69).NewYork:Appleton-Century-Crofts.Rouder,J.N.,Morey,R.D.,Speckman,P.L.,&Province,J.M.(2012).DefaultBayesfactorsforANOVAdesigns.JournalofMathematicalPsychology,56(5),356{374.Rouder,J.N.,Morey,R.D.,Verhagen,J.,Swagman,A.R.,&Wagenmakers,E.(2017).Bayesiananalysisoffactorialdesigns.PsychologicalMethods,22(2),304{321.Rouder,J.N.,Speckman,P.L.,Sun,D.,Morey,R.D.,&Iverson,G.(2009).Bayesianttestsforacceptingandrejectingthenullhypothesis.PsychonomicBulletin&Review,16(2),225{237.RStudio,T.(2015).RStudio:IntegratedDevelopmentEnvironmentforR.Boston,MA:RStudio,Inc.Retrievedfromhttps://rstudio.comRumelhart,D.E.,Hinton,G.E.,&Williams,R.J.(1986).Learningrepresentationsbyback-propagatingerrors.Nature,323(6088),533{536.167 Schultz,W.(2006).BehavioralTheoriesandtheNeurophysiologyofReward.AnnualReviewofPsychology,57(1),87{115.Schultz,W.,Dayan,P.,&Montague,P.R.(1997).Aneuralsubstrateofpredictionandreward.Science(NewYork,N.Y.),275(5306),1593{9.Shettleworth,S.J.(1972).ConstraintsonLearning.InD.S.Lehrman,R.A.Hinde,&E.Shaw(Eds.),Advancesinthestudyofbehavior(pp.1{68).AcademicPress.Skinner,B.F.(1948).\Superstition"inthepigeon.JournalofExperimentalPsychology,38(2),168{172.Spence,K.W.(1936).Thenatureofdiscriminationlearninginanimals.PsychologicalReview,43(5),427{449.Spence,K.W.(1937).Thedi erentialresponseinanimalstostimulivaryingwithinasingledimension.PsychologicalReview,44(5),430{444.Stout,S.C.,&Miller,R

.R.(2007).Sometimes-competingretrieval(SOCR):Aformal-izationofthecomparatorhypothesis.PsychologicalReview,114(3),759{783.Terrace,H.S.,Gibbon,J.,Farrell,L.,&Baldock,M.D.(1975).Temporalfactorsin uencingtheacquisitionandmaintenanceofanautoshapedkeypeck.AnimalLearning&Behavior,3(1),53{62.Thorndike,E.L.(1927).TheLawofE ect.TheAmericanJournalofPsychology,39(1/4),212.Timberlake,W.(1994).Behaviorsystems,associationism,andPavlovianconditioning.PsychonomicBulletin&Review,1(4),405{420.Timberlake,W.,Wahl,G.,&King,D.A.(1982).Stimulusandresponsecontingenciesinthemisbehaviorofrats.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalBehaviorProcesses,8(1),62{85.Urcelay,G.P.(2017).Competitionandfacilitationincompoundconditioning.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:AnimalLearningandCognition,43(4),303{314.Volkow,N.D.,Fowler,J.S.,Wang,G.,&Swanson,J.M.(2004).Featurereview:Dopamineindrugabuseandaddiction:resultsfromimagingstudiesandtreatment168 implications.MolecularPsychiatry,9,557{569.Wagner,A.R.(1981).SOP:Amodelofautomaticmemoryprocessinginanimalbehavior.InN.E.Spear&R.R.Miller(Eds.),Informationprocessinginanimals:Memorymechanisms(pp.5{148).Hillsdale,N.J.:Erlbaum.Wagner,A.R.,&Brandon,S.E.(1989).EvolutionofastructuredconnectionistmodelofPavlovianconditioning(SOP).InS.B.Klein&R.R.Mowrer(Eds.),Con-temporarylearningtheories:Pavlovianconditioningandthestatusoftraditionallearningtheories(pp.149{189).Hillsdale,N.J.:Erlbaum.Wagner,A.R.,Logan,F.A.,Haberlandt,K.,&Price,T.(1968).Stimulusselectioninanimaldiscriminationlearning.JournalofExperimentalPsychology,64,191{199.Wagner,A.R.,&Rescorla,R.A.(1972).InhibitioninPavlovianconditioning:Ap-plicationsofat

heory.InR.A.Boakes&M.S.Haliday(Eds.),Inhibitionandlearning(pp.301{336).NewYork:AcademicPress.Wasserman,E.A.(1974).Stimulus-reinforcerpredictivenessandselectivediscriminationlearninginpigeons.JournalofExperimentalPsychology,103(2),284{297.Wasserman,E.A.,Franklin,S.R.,&Hearst,E.(1974).Pavlovianappetitivecontin-genciesandapproachversuswithdrawaltoconditionedstimuliinpigeons.Journalofcomparativeandphysiologicalpsychology,86(4),616{27.Watson,J.B.(1924).Behaviorism.NewYoek:People'sInstitutePublishingCompany.Watt,A.,&Honey,R.C.(1997).CombiningCSsassociatedwiththesameordi er-entUSs.TheQuarterlyjournalofexperimentalpsychology.B,Comparativeandphysiologicalpsychology,50(4),350{67.Wickham,H.,Chang,W.,Henry,L.,LinPederse,T.,Takashashi,K.,Wike,C.,&Woo,K.(2018).package`ggplot2':CreateElegantDataVisualisationsUsingtheGrammarofGraphics.Retrievedfromhttps://ggplot2.tidyverse.orgWickham,H.,&Henry,L.(2018).package`tidyr':EasilyTidyDatawith`spread()'and`gather()'Functions.Retrievedfromhttps://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/tidyr/index.html169 Wickham,H.,Henry,L.,&Muller,K.(2019).package`dplyr':Agrammarofdatamanipulation.Retrievedfromhttps://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/dplyr/index.htmlWilliams,D.R.,&Williams,H.(1969).Auto-maintenanceinthepigeon:sustainedpeckingdespitecontingentnon-reinforcement.Journaloftheexperimentalanalysisofbehavior,12(4),511{20.Wilson,P.N.,Boumphrey,P.,&Pearce,J.M.(1992).RestorationoftheOrientingResponsetoaLightbyaChangeinitsPredictiveAccuracy:.QuarterlyJournalofExperimentalPsychology,44(B),17{36.Zimmer-Hart,C.L.,&Rescorla,R.A.(1974).ExtinctionofPavlovianconditionedinhibition.Journalofcomparativeandphysi

Related Contents


Next Show more