/
JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology,Vol.33,No.1,February2005,pp.55 JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology,Vol.33,No.1,February2005,pp.55

JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology,Vol.33,No.1,February2005,pp.55 - PDF document

natalia-silvester
natalia-silvester . @natalia-silvester
Follow
421 views
Uploaded On 2016-08-11

JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology,Vol.33,No.1,February2005,pp.55 - PPT Presentation

C 2005 DOI101007s108020050934z UnderstandingTeasingLessonsFromChildrenWithAutism ErinAHeerey 1 2 4 LisaMCapps 3 DacherKeltner 3 andAnnMKring 3 Teasingrequirestheabilitytounderstand ID: 442929

C  2005) DOI:10.1007/s10802-005-0934-z UnderstandingTeasing:LessonsFromChildrenWithAutism ErinA.Heerey 1  2  4 LisaM.Capps 3 DacherKeltner 3 andAnnM.Kring 3 Teasingrequirestheabilitytounderstand

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Pdf The PPT/PDF document "JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology,Vol.33,..." is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology,Vol.33,No.1,February2005,pp.55Ð68( C  2005) DOI:10.1007/s10802-005-0934-z UnderstandingTeasing:LessonsFromChildrenWithAutism ErinA.Heerey, 1  2  4 LisaM.Capps, 3 DacherKeltner, 3 andAnnM.Kring 3 Teasingrequirestheabilitytounderstandintention,nonliteralcommunication,pretense,andsocial context.ChildrenwithautismexperiencedifÞcultywithsuchskills,andconsequently,areexpected tohavedifÞcultywithteasing.Tobetterunderstandteasingconceptsandbehaviors,childrenwith autism,theirparents,andageandVerbal-IQ-matchedcomparisonchildrenandparentsdescribed conceptsandexperiencesofteasingandengagedinaparentÐchildteasinginteraction.Theteasingof childrenwithautismwaslessplayfulandprovocativeandfocusedlessonsocialnormsthanthatof onatheoryofmindtaskaccountedforseveraloftheobserveddifferences.Discussionfocusedonthe importanceofunderstandingsocialcontextandplayfulbehaviorduringteasing. KEYWORDS: autism;socialinteraction;theoryofmind;teasing. ÒTherearesomethingsIdonÕtknowsomuchabout.  Teasingisoneofthem.Ó ÐAparticipantwithautism,11yearsold. Teasingisacomplexyetvitalsocialinteraction throughwhichpeoplesocializeeachother,enterintoand maintainrelationships,andnegotiategroupmembership inganditisespeciallycommonamongfamilymembers andpeers.Anecdotally,teasingappearstobeespecially problematicforchildrenwithautism:theyteaseineffec- tivelyandseemtohavedifÞcultyunderstandingwhythey arebeingteased(Grandin,1995).Aconceptualanalysis ofteasingsuggestswhy.Thecomprehensionofteasing requiresformsofsocialunderstandingthatchildrenwith autismÞnddifÞcult,includingtheabilitytounderstand intention,nonliteralcommunication,pretense,andsocial Onthebasisofthisreasoning,wetestedhypothesesrelat- ingautismto(1)teasingbehavior,(2)recountedexperi- encesofteasing,and(3)positiveandnegativeconceptsof 1 MarylandPsychiatricResearchCenter,UniversityofMaryland- BaltimoreMedicalSchool,Baltimore,Maryland. 2 MentallllnessResearch,Education,andClinicalCenter,Departmentof VeteransÕAffairsMarylandHealthCareSystem,Baltimore,Maryland. 3 PsychologyDepartment,UniversityofCalifornia,Berkeley,California. 4 (Suite6A),10NorthGreeneStreet,Baltimore,Maryland21201; e-mail:eheerey@mprc.umaryland.edu. teasing.High-functioningchildrenwithautismandtheir parentsandcomparisonchildrenandtheirparentspartic- ipatedinaninterviewaboutteasingconceptsandexperi- encesaswellasaparentÐchildteasinginteractionwherein theyinventednicknamesforoneanother. DeÞnitionsofTeasing ThetermÒteasingÓhasbeenappliedtomyriadsocial behaviors,rangingfromhostilebullying(Smith&Brain, 2000),totheaffectionate,playfulidiomsofromanticpart- geneouscategoryofbehaviors,wehavedeÞnedteasingas aprovocationthatcommentsonsomethingofrelevance tothetarget(Keltneretal.,2001).Theprovocationcan beverbal(e.g.,aninsultorcommentondeviantbehavior) ornonverbal(e.g.,apokeintheribs).Toreducethehos- tilityofthisprovocation,theteasermayemployplayful orÒoff-recordÓmarkers,whichconveythattheprovoca- tionistobetakenpartlyinthespiritofplay.Off-record markersincludeverbalcomments(e.g.,ÒjustkiddingÓ), repetition,exaggeration),andprosodiccues(e.g.,singÐ songvoice)thatsignalthattheprovocationisnotentirely serious.Teasingthen,isaformofprovocation(criticism orhostility)mitigatedbyoff-recordmarkers(playfulges- tures),eachofwhichmaybepresenttovaryingdegrees. Thistypeofteasingisconsideredprosocial,inthatits 55 0091-0627/05/0200-0055/0 C  2005SpringerScience  BusinessMedia,Inc. 56 Heerey,Capps,Keltner,andKring generalaimistocommentuponorcorrectanaspectof thetarget Õ ssocialbehavior. SocialUnderstandingandTeasing Althoughitmaybeparsedinotherways,ourde Þ - nitionsuggeststhatthegenerationandcomprehensionof teasinghingesonseveralabilities.Elementaltotheunder- standingofteasingarefairlysophisticatedabilitiesinthe comprehensionofintention.Inprovokinginplayfulfash- ion,theteaseconveysbothintenttocriticize,andplayful, affectionateintent.Theeffectiveteaserthen,mustbeable toconveythesecon ß ictingintentionsandtherecipientto decipherthem,oftenduringrelativelybrief,emotionally chargedexchanges. Teasingalsorequiresadroitnessinnonliteralcom- munication.Muchoftheplayfulcontentofateaseisnon- literal,seeninthesmiles,prosodicvariations(e.g.,sing Ð songvoice),andgrammaticaldevices(e.g.,exaggeration) thatindirectlyrendertheprovocationlesshostile.Thus,in understandingteasing,onemustinfertheimpliedmean- ingsbasedonthejuxtapositionoftheliteralprovocation andthenonliteralmeaninginasetofsubtleparalinguistic acts. Teasingfurtherinvolveselementsofpretensethat,to beunderstoodproperly,requiretheabilitytoassumepre- tendrolesandstances(forelaborationonthisnotion,see Clark,1996;Clark&Gerrlg,1984;Keltneretal.,2001). Thatis,teasingconveyscoexistingliteralandnonliteral representationsofthetargetofatease,whichnecessitate thatindividualsdifferentiaterealityfrompretense(Leslie, 1987).Asinotherformsofostensiblecommunication,the teaserandtargetassumehypotheticalidentities,andthe teaserdeliversaprovocationtothetarget.Thehypothet- icalteaserintendstheprovocationtobeserious,andthe hypotheticaltargetreceivesitassuch.Theactualtarget however,isexpectedtodiscoverthepretenseandtounder- standtheactualteaser Õ sattitudetowardthehypothetical teaser,thehypotheticaltarget,andtheprovocation(Clark &Gerrig,1984).Theabilitytoproduceandinterpretacts ofpretenseisthereforecriticaltoteasing. Finally,teasingtypicallycommentsuponsomede- viationfromthesocialnorm(e.g.,Keltner,Young,Heerey, Oemig,&Monarch,1998;Shapiro,Baumeister,&Kessler, 1991).Socialnormsconstitutethesetofexpectationsthat governbehaviorandinteractionsbothamonggroupmem- bers(e.g.,fraternitybrothers,romanticpartners,family members)andbetweenthegroupandtheoutsideworld. Thenormrelatedfocusofmuchteasingpoints,somewhat ironically,totheprosocialendsofteasing:inteasing,the teaserimpliesaninterestinthetarget Õ sengagementin sociallyappropriatebehavior(Bell,Buerkel-Rothfuss,& Gore,1987;Eder,1993;Eisenberg,1986).Thus,under- standingsocialnormsandtheactionsthatviolatethemis acentralelementintheprovocationofatease. Childrenunderstandtheprovocativenatureofteas- ingearlyon,evidentintheiruniversallynegativeviews thereof.Byages10 Ð 11however,theyhavebeguntocon- ceptualizeteasingasmorepositiveandprosocial,andto teasebothmoreplayfullyandprovocatively,whilecon- tinuingtoreportnegativefeelingsaboutteasing(Warm, 1997).Thisdevelopmentalshiftislikelyrelatedtothe engagementoftheseinterrelatedabilities,uponwhich wehavearguedprosocialteasinghinges.Childrenwith autismhavedif Þ cultieswithsuchskills,suggestingthat thepositivesideofteasingwillproveelusive. AutismandtheUnderstandingofIntention Ahallmarkofautisticdisorderisdif Þ cultywiththe- oryofmind,theabilitytounderstandmentalstates,such astheintentionsofothers(Flavell,1999).Theoryofmind hasbeenlinkedtoformsofspeechandsocialreasoning thatpresupposeanunderstandingofothers Õ mentalstates (Frith,Happe,&Siddons,1994;Happe,1993).Thesetof abilitiesthatweproposetounderlieteasingareallaspects oftheoryofmind. Higherfunctioningchildrenwithautismhavedif Þ - cultyinusingacharacter Õ sprobablementalstateorinten- tiontoexplainthecharacter Õ sambiguousspeechorbehav- ior(Abell,Happe,&Frith,2000;Tager-Flusberg,1999). Forexample,inHappe Õ s Ò StrangeStories Ó task,children wereaskedtointerpretshortsocialvignettesinwhichan ambiguousstatementwasmade.Inonevignette,achild coughsthroughoutlunch.Herfathersays, Ò PoorEmma. Youmusthaveafroginyourthroat. Ó Relativetocom- parisonchildren,childrenwithautismweremorelikely toconcludethatsuchstatementswereliterallytrueand usedfewerexplanationsinvolvingmentalstates(Happe, 1994).Thiskindofevidencesuggeststhatchildrenwith autismwillhavedif Þ cultydiscerningtheplayfulintentof ateasethatistypicallyconveyedinanambiguous,nonlit- eralfashion.Thisdiminishedcapacitytorecognizesocial intentionsmayservetomakethetruemeaningofatease remainopaque. NonliteralCommunicationandChildrenWithAutism Autismisassociatedwithahostofdif Þ cultiesinthe realmofcommunication(Tager-Flusberg,1999),includ- ingdif Þ cultiesdecipheringnonliterallanguage(Perner, UnderstandingTeasing 57 Frith,Leslie,&Leekam,1989).Childrenwithautism tendtoexplainsocialcommunicationsusingsystemsof interpretationthatrelyontheliteralmeaningofanut- terance(Hobson,1989).Forexample,individualswith autismfrequentlyhavedif Þ cultyinterpretingspeechacts thatrelyonthelistener Õ simplicitunderstandingofgen- eralsocialconventionsorpriorknowledge(e.g., Ò Doyou knowwhattimeitis? Ó ).Despitethefactthatthespeaker hasnotspeci Þ callyinquiredaboutthetime,thelistener isexpectedtounderstandthatthetimeofdayisbeing requestedandtorespondaccordingly.Individualswith autismmayrespondliterallytotheutterance(e.g., Ò Yes. Iknowwhattimeitis. Ó ),ratherthantothenonliteral,in- directmeaningthespeakerhasimplied(Bara,Bosco,& Bucciarelli,1999;Frith,1989). Theuseofidiomaticlanguage,anothervarietyof nonliteralcommunicationthatiscommontoteasing,par- ticularlyintheuseofnicknames(Bell&Healey,1992), hasbeenshowntobedif Þ cultforindividualswithautism. Inonestudy,agroupofhigh-functioningadultswith autismshowedperformancede Þ cits,relativetoacompar- isongroup,onataskinwhichtheyidenti Þ edidioms(e.g., Ò potluck, ÓÒ fatchance, ÓÒ pointblank, Ó etc.,Strandburg, Marsh,Brown,&Asarnow,1993).Sarcasmisalsodif Þ - cultforchildrenwithautism.Insteadofattendingtothe voicetoneandprosodyusedindelivery,childrenwith autismarelikelytoerroneouslyrespondonlytothever- balcontentofaremark,missingitsintendedmeaning (Frith,1989).Becausetheintendedmeaningofatease iscommonlyconveyedinanonliteralorindirectfashion (Keltneretal.,2001),teaseinterpretationislikelytobe problematicforchildrenwithautism. Pretense,Play,andChildrenWithAutism Spontaneouspretendplay,commonamongtoddlers andyoungchildren,appearstopresentdif Þ cultyforindi- vidualswithautism.Althoughchildrenwithautismhave beenshowntoengageinpretendplay,inparticularwhen initiatedbyothers(Jarrold,Boucher,&Smith,1993),they arelesslikelytospontaneouslyuseobjectsinimaginative waysandtendtopreferliteralplaytothatinvolvingpre- tense(Charman&Baron-Cohen,1997).Moreover,they havebeenshowntoengageinlessspontaneousfunctional playandimitativeplaythantheirpeers(Jarroldetal., 1993).Insocialinteraction,de Þ citsinspontaneousplay maymanifestasadearthinnonverbalgesturesandplayful verbalandnonverbalactions(Mundy,Sigman,&Kasari, 1990).Becauseteasingcanbeviewedasaformofsocial pretense,childrenwithautismmay Þ nditsplayfulaspects bothdif Þ culttoinitiateandtounderstand. UnderstandingofSocialContext AmongChildrenWithAutism Asteasingbecomesmoresophisticateditincreas- inglyrevolvesaroundsocialization(Keltneretat.,2001). Childrenwithautismhavebeenshowntobelesssen- sitivetothesocialenvironmentthancomparisonchil- dren(Capps&Sigman,1996).Forinstance,individuals withautismseemtobemoreinclinedtoresistfollow- ingnormsgoverningsocialcomportment(Frith,1989; Szatmari,Offord,Siegel,&Finlayson,1990),whichmay stemfromdif Þ cultyinidentifyinginappropriatebehav- iors.Instudiesofjudgmentsofbehaviors,childrenwith autismhadmoredif Þ cultyidentifyingandexplainingin- appropriatesocialbehaviors(Loveland,Pearson,Tunali- Kotoski,Ortegon,&Gibbs,2001),andfauxpasthan didcomparisonparticipants(Baron-Cohen,O Õ Riordan, Stone,Jones,&Plaisted,1999).Thus,someofthesocial de Þ citsmanifestedinchildrenwithautismmaystemfrom theirapparentlackofknowledgeaboutimportantsocial norms,implyingdif Þ cultyinunderstandingteasing. CURRENTRESEARCH Thedevelopmentalliteraturesuggeststhatwithin- creasedsocialunderstanding,teasingbecomesmoresym- bolicandplayful(Warm,1997),morefocusedonsocial norms(Keltneretal.,2001),andmorepositive(Shapiro etat.,1991).Inconcreteterms,thisliteraturesuggests thatwhereasthehostile,provocativecomponentofteas- ingvariesonlyslightlyacrosslevelofsocialunderstand- ing,themoreplayful,prosocialcomponentsofteasing increaseinvolumeandsophisticationwithsocialdevel- opment,promptingchangeinindividuals Õ interpretations ofteasing(Lightner,Bollmer,Harris,Milich,&Scambler, 2000).Onthebasisofourde Þ nitionofteasingandwhat isknownaboutautism-relateddif Þ cultiesinsocialunder- standing,weexpectboththeunderstandingandgeneration ofteasingtobeproblematicforchildrenwithautism. Asa Þ nalnoteaboutteasing,parentsplayanim- portantandearlyroleinchildren Õ steasing(Keltneretal., 2001).Parentsofchildrenwithautismhaveagreatdealof concernabouttheteasingtheirchildrenrelateand,anec- dotally,attempttosoftenthestingofthisoftenpainful interaction(Barron&Barron,1992).However,parentsof childrenwithautismreportoccasionalboutsoffriendly, ritualizedteasingorjokingwiththeirchildren,oftencen- teredondailyroutines(McDonnell,1993).Itistherefore likelythatparentsofchildrenwithautism,likethoseof, typicallydevelopingchildren,adapttheirteasingtomeet thecapabilitiesoftheirchildren. 58 Heerey,Capps,Keltner,andKring METHODS Aspartofalargerstudyofsocialunderstanding, childrenwithhigh-functioningautism,typicallydevelop- ingchildren,andtheirparentswereinterviewedabouttheir conceptsaswellastheirexperienceswithteasing.Inaddi- tion,parentsandtheirchildrenengagedinadyadicteasing interactioninwhichtheyinventedandsharednicknames foroneanother. Participants Participantsincluded43children:23nonretarded childrenwithdiagnosesofautism( n  10)orAsperger Õ s syndrome( n  13)comprisedthehigh-functioning autism/Asperger Õ ssyndrome(HFA)group,and20typi- callydevelopingchildrenmadeupthecomparisongroup. HFAparticipantswererecruitedthroughclinicians,none ofwhomwereaf Þ liatedwiththeproject.Tocon Þ rmdi- agnosesofeitherautismorAsperger Õ ssyndrome,one orbothparentsofHFAchildrenwereadministeredthe AutismDiagnosticInterview-Revised(ADI-R;Lord, Rutter,&LeCouteur,1994),whichgeneratesdiagnoses basedon DSM-IV criteria.TheADI-Rachievesreliable diagnosesofautismandAsperger Õ ssyndromeacrossgen- dersandagegroups(forspeci Þ c Þ ndings,seePilowsky, Yirmiya,Shulman,&Dover,1998).Nosigni Þ cantdif- ferencesemergedbetweenparticipantswithautismand thosewithAsperger Õ ssyndromeonanymeasuredvari- ables.Thus,analyses(below)didnotdistinguishpartici- pantsbydiagnosis. OnlychildrenwhosecurrentVerbal,Performance, andFullScaleIQscores,asmeasuredusingtheWechsler IntelligenceScaleforChildren,EditionIII(WISC-III; Wechsler,1991),were80orhigherwereincludedinthe study.Acutoffof80ensuredthatstudyparticipantswere withinthenormalrangeontheWISC-IIIandthatnochild hadmentalretardation.AllHFAchildrenwereworkingat ageappropriategrade-levelsand21ofthe23children werebeingeducatedinmainstreamedschoolenviron- ments.Twoattendedaprivateschoolforchildrenwith learningdisabilities.AutismandAsperger Õ ssyndromeaf- fectboysathigherratesthangirls(Capps&Sigman, 1996).Thisbiaswasre ß ectedinourHFAsample,18of whomwereboys. Comparisonchildrenwererecruitedfromlocal schoolsandrecreationprogramsinalargemetropolitan area.Asindicatedduringatelephonescreening,noneof thechildrenhadpriorpsychiatrichistoriesnorwerethey beingtreatedforpsychologicaldif Þ cultiesofanykind. Inaddition,thepresenceofaPervasiveDevelopmental Disorderwasruledoutusingaparent-reportquestion- naire,thePervasiveDevelopmentalDisorderScreening Test(Siegel,1986).Groupswerestatisticallymatchedon verbalIQ,chronologicalage,andgender(foradiscus- sionofageandverbalIQmatchinginstudiesofhigh- functioningautismseeOzonoff,Rogers,&Pennington, 1991).TherewerenogroupdifferencesonVerbal IQ( M HFA  103  82(14  86); M Control  106  00(7  97); t (38)   86  ns ),fromtheWISC-III.Allchildrenwere aged8to15years,andthegroupsdidnotdifferonage, ( M HFA  10  92(2  62); M Control  10  57(1  21); t (38)  1  18  ns )orgender(HFA:5girls;18boys;Control:4girls; 16boys;  2 (1)  0  75  ns ).Allchildrenreceived$10/hr inappreciationoftheirparticipation. Procedure Testingwascompletedintwo2-hrsessions,typi- callyoccurring5to7daysapart,duringwhichchildren completedtheWISC-IIIandabatteryoftasksassessing socialunderstanding,emotion,theoryofmind,andnar- rativecompetence.SessionswerevideotapedusingPana- sonicvideocamerasthatwerevisibletoparticipants.All participantswereseatedatatableacrossfromanexperi- menterandcameraswerepositionedsuchthatthepartic- ipant Õ sface,upperbody,andaportionofthetablewere inviewatalltimes.Oneofthetaskspresentedduringthe Þ rststudysession,aninterviewaboutteasing,generated datarelevanttothepresentinvestigation.Aparent Ð child teasinginteraction(adaptedfromKeltneretal.,1998)was conductedduringthesecondstudysessionandthesedata werealsoincludedinthepresentstudy.Finally,atheory ofmindtask,StrangeStories(Happe,1994),wasadmin- isteredtochildrenforthepurposesofrelatingtheoryof mindtochildren Õ steasingbehaviorsandconcepts. Parent Ð ChildTeasingInteraction Childrenandtheirparentswereseatedside-by-side, acrossatablefromtwoexperimenters.Theexperimenters, bothofwhomhadworkedcloselywithchildrenandtheir parentsduringthestudysessions,introducedandmod- eledthetask.Participantsweretoldthattheywouldbe playinga Ò nicknamegame Ó inwhichtheywouldeachbe askedtoinventanicknamefortheotherandexplainthat nickname.Toallowparticipantsto Ò warm-up Ó totheinter- actionsetting,theywereaskedtodescribeanynicknames theyhadforoneanotherathomeandwhetherothermem- bersoftheirfamilieshadnicknames.Experimentersthen explainedthattheywouldinventnicknamesforonean- other,whichcouldberealorfanciful,andprovideabrief UnderstandingTeasing 59 rationaleforeachnickname.Noconstraintswereplaced onthegenerationofnicknames.Experimenter1demon- stratedthegamebyteasingExperimenter2(i.e., Ò Iwould callyou,T.F.forTomatoFacebecauseofthewayyou blushwhenyougetcallednicknames. Ó ).Experimenter2 likewisedemonstratedtheprocedurebyinventinganick- nameforthechild(i.e., Ò IwouldcallyouS.P.,forSmarty Pantsbecauseyouweresosmartinourstudytoday. Ó ). Allparticipantsheardthesametwointroductorynick- names.Participants Õ questionswereansweredandchil- drenwerepromptedtodelivertheirnicknames(e.g., Ò Can youthinkofanicknameforyourmom? Ó ).Childrende- liveredtheirnicknamesand,iftheydidnotspontaneously generatearationaleforthenickname,werepromptedto doso(e.g., Ò Whywouldyoucallherthat? Ó ).Parentswere thenasked,usingsimilarprompts,toinventandexplaina nicknameforthechild. Ifachildwasunabletogeneratethe Þ rstnickname, aswasthecasewith13childrenacrossthegroups,the parentwaspromptedtoproduceandexplainanickname forthechild.Followingtheparent Õ sdelivery,thechild wasagainpromptedtoinventanicknamefortheparent. Ifthechildwasstillunabletogenerateanickname(two comparisonchildrenandfourHFAchildren),thegame wasconcludedwithoutthechild Õ stease.Noparentwas unabletogenerateanickname. ChildandParentTeasingInterviews TeasingConcepts Teasinginterviewsforbothchildrenandparentsbe- gansimilarly.Participantswereasked, Ò Howdoyoude Þ ne teasing? Ó Iftheparticipantwasunsureaboutthemean- ingofthequestionorunabletoanswer,theexperimenter providedasecondprompt: Ò Whenyouthinkofteasing, whatcomestomind? Ó Allparticipantsunderstoodandan- sweredthesecondprompt,providingthreeto Þ vedifferent ideasaboutteasing. TeasingExperiences Followingthepromptsforconceptsofteasing,par- ticipantswereaskedtorecountteasingexperiencesbyde- scribingtimeswhentheyteasedandwereteasedbyothers. Speci Þ cally,childrenwereaskedtodescribeoneincident ofteasingwhentheyteased,andoneincidentofteasing whentheywereteasedbypeersatschool,parentsathome, and,ifapplicable,siblingsathome.Thus,childrenwith siblingswerepromptedtorecountatotalofsixteasing episodes:(1)asthetargetofpeerteasing,(2)whenteas- ingapeer,(3)asthetargetofaparent Õ steasing,(4)when teasingaparent,(5)asthetargetofsiblingteasing,and (6)whenteasingasibling.Childrenwithoutsiblingswere askedtorecountfourteasingepisodes,excludingprompts aboutsiblingteasing.Parentswereaskedtorecallatime inwhich(1)theyteasedthechildinthestudyand(2)the childinthestudyteasedthem.Parentsofmorethanone childwereadditionallypromptedtodescribeteasingin- teractionswitheachoftheirotherchildren,astheyhad with,thechildinthestudy.Participantswereaskedto provideaccountsoftheirteasinginteractionsbutwerenot speci Þ callypromptedfordetails(e.g., Ò Tellmeabouta timewhenyouwereteasedbyyourbrother.Tellmewhat happened. Ó or Ò Tellmeaboutatimewhenyouteasedyour momordad.Tellmewhathappened. Ó ). StrangeStoriesTask Atheoryofmindmeasure,consistingof12short vignettesinwhichstorycharactersproduceambiguous speechoractions(e.g.,achild,playingwithafriend,picks upabananafromafruitbowl,holdsittoherearandsays, Ò Look!Thisbananaisatelephone! Ó ),wasadministered (foradescriptionofthe Ò strangestories Ó theoryofmind taskaswellasscoringinformationandrelated Þ ndings seeHappe,1994).Theexperimenterreadeachvignette aloudtothechildandthenaskedthechildtoanswertwo questionsaboutthestory: Ò Wasittrue,what[astorychar- acter]said? Ó and Ò Whydid[thestorycharacter]saythat? Ó Positivecommentsweremadeduringtestingbutchildren weregivennofeedbackaboutwhethertheiranswerswere correct. CodingofChild Ð ParentTeasingInteractions Teasinginteractionswerecodedfromvideotapefor thefollowingitems: TeaseClassi Þ cation Eachteasewasclassi Þ edaccordingtotypeofteasing: socialnormviolation (ateaserelatingtoasocialnorm, e.g., Ò pant-a-balloon Ó forachildwhowears Ò pantsthat look5sizestoobig Ó ), characterteasing (ateaserelating toaconsistentaspectofanindividual Õ sbeing,e.g., Ò little go-go Ó forachildwho Ò isalwaysonthegoandnever seemstogettired Ó ),or endearingnickname (oftenacur- rentnicknameoftheparentoranicknameofthechild duringinfancy,e.g., Ò shin-shin Ó meaning Ò littlestar Ó in Chinese).Itwasseldomapparentfromthenicknamealone 60 Heerey,Capps,Keltner,andKring howateaseshouldbeclassi Þ ed.Therefore,inallcases bothnicknameandrationalewereusedinclassi Þ cation. Ninety-twopercentofteaseswereuniquelyclassi Þ edinto thethreecategories.Theremainingnineteases,allgen- eratedbychildren(twocomparisonandsevenHFA),in- cludednicknamesthatwereeitherentirelynonverbal,or wereacombinationofnonverbalgestures(e.g.,sticking outtongue)andverbalizedsoundsthatdidnotinclude words(e.g.,snif ß ing,sighing).Sixofthenineteasesin- cludedverbalrationales.Theseteases,includingthenick- nameandrationale,wereclassi Þ edandanalyzedforboth verbalcontentandnonverbalbehavior(seebelow).The remaining3teases,allgeneratedbyHFAchildren,con- tainednoverbalcontentandwereanalyzedfornonverbal behavioronly. VerbalContent Theverbalcontentofeachteasewasratedontwo, unipolar,7-pointLikertscalesaccordingtohow af Þ liative andhow critical itwasoftherecipient(1  notatall af Þ liative/critical ;7  extremelyaf Þ liative/critical ).Af- Þ liativeteaseswerethoseinwhichtheverbalcontentof eitherthenicknameorrationaleincludedpraise,endear- ment,referencestopositivebehaviors,etc.Forexample, onechildwasnicknamed Ò Super-Student Ó byhismother duetoastraight-Areportcard.Thisteasewasconsidered af Þ liativebecausetheteasecenteredonapositivetopic, beingagoodstudent,andinvolvedpraise.Criticalteases involvedtopicssuchasnegativeaspectsoftherecipient Õ s character,accidents,mistakes,orteasesinwhichthere- cipientwasaccusedofuntowardbehavior.Forexample, onechildcalledherfather Ò CreativeFarts Ó duetopublic ß atulence. PlayfulandCriticalBehaviors Onthebasisofasummaryoftheliteratureregard- ingnonverbalmarkersofteasing(Keltneretal.,2001), wecodedplayfulbehaviors,includingfriendlylaughter, smiles,playfulprosody(sing Ð songvoice),playfulges- tures(waving),reassuringphysicalcontact(hand-holding, pattingtheinteractionpartner),andplayfulmimicry(play- fulparodyofanactionorotherpersondesignedtopro- motelaughter).Criticalbehaviorsincludedfrowns,sneers, grimaces,politesmiles,criticalprosody(e.g.,sarcastic tones),criticalgestures(e.g.,stickingouttongue),aggres- sivephysicalcontactoractions(e.g.,slapping,poking, pretendingtohitanother),andhostilemimicry(e.g.,par- odyofanactionorotherdesignedtomock).Foreach tease,wesummedthenumberofobservationsofplayful, off-recordmarkersandthenumberofcriticalgesturesto generateatotalnumberofplayfulandcriticalbehaviors perparticipant. CodingTeasingInterviews Interviewswerecodedfromvideotapesforverbal content.Unlessotherwisenoted,alldatageneratedwere frequencycounts. TeasingConcepts Wecodedparticipants Õ conceptsofteasingaseither (1) positive :iftheresponseindicatedthatteasingcould beviewedasenjoyable(e.g., Ò teasingisplayful, ÓÒ joking around, ÓÒ fun Ó )or(2) negative :iftheresponseindicated thatteasingwasunpleasant(e.g., Ò teasingisbeingmean, Ó Ò bullying, ÓÒ hurtingsomeone Õ sfeelings Ó ).Eachpartici- pantspontaneouslygeneratedatleasttwodistinctconcepts ofteasingthatwerecodedaseitherpositiveornegative. Thetotalnumberofnegativeconceptsre ß ectedthenum- berofuniquenegativestatementsparticipantsmadeabout teasing.Totalpositiveconceptsweresimilarlycalculated. TeasingExperiences Wecodedparticipants Õ recountedteasingexperiences asfollows: SocialContext Recountedteasingexperienceswerecodedforthe totalnumberofreferencesmadeineachofthefollow- ingdomains:(1) antecedents :orwhytheteasehappened, werecodedfromdescriptionsoftheactionsleadingupto theteaseitself(e.g., Ò Ikeptmissingmyshotatbasket- ball  Ó );and(2) consequences :theresultsofthetease, werecodedfromdescriptionsoftheoutcomeofatease (e.g., Ò  soIconcentratedhardandmadealltherestof myshotsexceptone,then[theteaser]couldn Õ tsayany- thingaboutthatanymore. Ó ). Valence Theverbalcontentofateasecanbeclassi Þ edasei- therpositiveornegativeinvalenceaccordingtowhether theteaseisaboutpositiveornegativebehaviorsortraits (seeKeltneretal.,1998).Forexample,achildwhore- portsbeingteasedaboutoutstandingathleticoracademic abilityisbeingteasedaboutapositiveability,eventhough UnderstandingTeasing 61 thechildmaynotreportfeelingpositiveduringthetease. Teasingwithnegativecontentinvolvesnegativetraitsor behaviors,suchasirritability,asinthecaseofachild teasedaboutpicking Þ ghtswithasibling.Recountedteas- ingexperienceswereclassi Þ edaseither(1) positive or (2) negative incontentvalence.Valencewascodedinde- pendentlyofthechild Õ sinterpretationofthetease. TeasingCategories Teasingexperienceswereclassi Þ edintofourcate- gories(seeWarm,1997).Teasesaboutviolationsofso- cialnorms,e.g.,thingssomeone does suchasdroppinga lunchtrayintheschoolcafeteria,wereclassi Þ edas social normviolations .Teasingexperiencesthatfocusedonan individual Õ sphysicalorpsychologicalcharacter,suchas apersonalitytrait(beingverynice),physicaltrait(hav- ingalargenose),ormentalcharacteristic(beingforget- ful),wereclassi Þ edas characterteasing .Descriptions ofmimicry,namecalling,mocking,andphysicalteasing (poking,hitting,touching)werecategorizedas taunting . Practicaljokes,pranks,tricks,whitelies,anddescriptions ofsituationsinwhichapersonisledtobelievesomething falsewereclassi Þ edas trickery .Allbuttwooftheteasing descriptions(about99%)wereclassi Þ edintooneofthe fourcategories. Forthethreemeasuresofrecountedexperiences Ñ socialcontext,valence,andteasecategory Ñ wesummed thescoresacrossallrecountedexperiencesforeachmea- sureandthendividedbythenumberofexperiencesthatthe participantrecounted.Inourdataanalysis,wecollapsed acrossteaserandtargetexperiences,becauseanumberof HFAchildren(7)didnotrecountatimewhentheyteased someone. CodingofStrangeStories Children Õ sresponsestothestrangestoriestaskwere codedasfollows.Childrenwhogeneratedcorrectanswers tothe Þ rstquestion( Ò Wasittrue,what[storycharac- ter]said? Ó ),andexplainedtheiranswerswithreference tothesocialprocessofthestory,received2points.Those whogeneratedcorrectanswerstothe Þ rstquestionbut explainedtheiranswersusingincorrectmentalorsocial processesortheperceptualfeaturesofthestoryreceived ascoreof1point.Childrenwhoansweredincorrectly andexplainedtheiranswersreferringtonomentalorso- cialprocessesorsaid, Ò Idon Õ tknow, Ó receivedscores of0.Scoresweresummed,yieldingamaximumscore of24points(forcompleteresults,seeSobel,Capps& Gopnik,1999). CodingandReliability Allcoderswereblindtobothparticipants Õ groupsta- tusandtoexperimentalhypotheses.Agroupoffourun- dergraduateresearchassistantsweretrainedtoclassify teasesandcodeplayfulandhostilecontentandbehavior. Eachratercodedtwointeractionsunderthesupervision ofthe Þ rstauthorandworkedindependentlythereafter. Excludingthesixchildrenwhowereunabletogenerate nicknames,atotalof80teasesweregeneratedandcoded duringtheinteraction. Toassessreliability,eachteasewasindependently codedbytworaters.Thereliabilityofraters Õ teaseclassi- Þ cationswasassessedusingakappacoef Þ cient(    84). Intraclasscorrelationcoef Þ cientswerecalculatedforrat- ingsofverbalcontentaswellasparticipants Õ nonverbal behavior.Theserangedfrom.64to.91.Analyseswere conductedusingadatasetthatre ß ected,foreachtease, theaverageofthetworaters Õ codes. Twoundergraduateresearchassistants,notassoci- atedwiththecodingoftheteasinginteractions,worked tocodeteasinginterviews.Eachcodedseveralinterviews underthesupervisionofthe Þ rstauthor,andthereafter workedindividuallyfromvideotapestogeneratedatarel- evanttotheinvestigationofteasinginterviews.Coders overlappedtheircodingsuchthatabouthalfoftheinter- views(46)werecodedindependentlybytwocoders. Forthechild-teasinginterviews,theintraclasscorre- lationsbetweencoders Õ ratingsofitemsrelatingtocon- ceptsandexperiencesofteasing,includingde Þ nitions, antecedents,consequences,socialcontext,functions,and nonverbalbehaviorsrangedfrom.69to.89.Inaddition, eachteasingepisodewasclassi Þ edaccordingtoitscon- tentcategory:socialnormviolation,characterteasing, taunting,ortrickery(    76).Forparent-teasinginter- views,theintraclasscorrelationsbetweencoders Õ ratings ofitemsrelatingtoconceptsandexperiencesofteasing rangedfrom.71to.94.Classi Þ cationofparent-reported teasingepisodesaccordingtocontentcategoriesyielded akappacoef Þ cientof.73. Tworaterscodedthetheoryofminddatafromtran- scripts.Thesecoderswereindependentofthosewhohad codedtheteasinginteractionsandinterviews.Theyat- tainedahighlevelofagreement(    87). RESULTS Becauseoftheinteractivenatureofthetask,teas- inginteractionswereanalyzedbydyad.Weconducted amixedmodelANOVA,treatinggroup(HFA,compari- son)asthebetween-dyadvariableandparticipant(parent, 62 Heerey,Capps,Keltner,andKring child)asthewithin-dyadvariable.Posthoctestscompar- ingchildrenfromthetwogroupsandparentsfromthetwo groupswereconductedonsigni Þ cantomnibus F susing theNewman-KeulscorrectionfortypeIerrorrates(Glass &Hopkins,1996).Forteasinginterviews,parents Õ and children Õ sdatawereanalyzedseparately,againusingthe Newman-Keulsmethodtocontrolerrorrates. TeasingBehavior Wehypothesizedthatchildrenwithautismandtheir parentswouldhaveparticulardif Þ cultygeneratingthe playfulcomponentofteasing,whichhingesontheuse ofnonliteral,off-recordmarkersandpretense.Consistent withthishypothesis,amixedmodelANOVAfoundthat comparisonchildrenandtheirparentsusedmoreplayful behaviors,suchassmiles,unusualintonation,andexag- geratedgesturesintheirteasingthandidchildrenwith autismandtheirparents, F (1  40)  8  06; p  01(see TableIformeans).Morespeci Þ canalysesrevealedthat comparisonchildrenusedmoreplayfulbehaviorsthandid HFAchildren, t (38)  2  42; p  05,asdidcomparison groupparentswhencomparedwithHFAgroupparents, t (38)  2  56; p  05.Parentsandchildrendidnotdiffer intheextenttowhichtheyincorporatedplayfulbehaviors intotheirteasing, F (1  40)   90; ns . Givenanecdotalevidencesuggestingthatprovoca- tiveteasingisproblematicforchildrenwithautism,we expectedHFAgroupparticipantstoteaseinmoreaf Þ l- iative,lesscriticalwaysthancomparisongrouppartici- pants.Mixed-modelANOVAsdemonstratedthatcompar- isonparticipantswere(1)lessaf Þ liative, F (1  40)  6  64; p  01,and(2)morecritical, F (1  40)  8  93; p  01, intheirteasingthanwereHFAparticipants.Thesesame analysesrevealedthatregardlessofgroup,parentswere TableI. ContentandBehaviorinTestingInteractions ChildrenParents HFA( N  23)Comparison( N  20)HFA( N  23)Comparison( N  20) Nonverbalbehaviors Playful1.61(1.23)2.27(1.08)1.39(1.15)2.07(1.19) Critical0.36(0.88)0.17(0.43)0.25(0.53)0.13(0.38) Teasingcontent Af Þ liative3.65(1.51)3.38(1.40)5.20(1.23)4.20(1.66) Critical2.39(1.75)3.27(1.53)1.36(1.07)2.54(1.48) Teasingtypes Socialnorms0.41(0.68)0.68(0.65)0.48(0.67)0.93(10.79) Character0.24(0.54)0.11(0.38)0.32(0.58)0.42(0.75) Endearingnames0.39(0.49)0.09(0.29)0.40(0.50)0.36(0.49) Note .Allvariablesarelistedasmeanspertease.Standarddeviationsareinparentheses. lesscriticaloftheirchildrenthanchildrenwereoftheir parents, F (1  40)  8  67; p  01,andalsomoreaf Þ lia- tivewhileteasingthanwerechildren, F (1  40)  17  50; p  001.Morefocusedcomparisonsrevealedthatchil- drenintheHFAgroupdidnotteaseinmoreaf Þ liativefash- ionthancomparisonchildren, t (38)  0  95; ns ,although parentsofHFAchildrenwereindeedmoreaf Þ liativein theirteasingthancomparisonparents, t (38)  2  70; p   01.Comparisonchildrendid,however,teaseinmorecrit- icalwaysthandidHFAchildren, t (38)  2  01; p  05. Likewise,comparisongroupparentsweremorecriticalof theirchildrenthanwereHFAgroupparents, t (38)  3  40; p  01. Intheanalysisofcriticalbehaviors,includingcritical gestures,frowns,andsarcasticintonation,mixedmodel ANOVAyieldedneitherdifferencesbetweentheHFA andcomparisongroups, F (1  40)  0  89  ns ,norbetween parentsandchildren, F (1  40)  0  17; ns  Wenowturntothenormativecontentoftheteas- ing.WehadpredictedthatHFAchildrenandtheirpar- entswouldbelesslikelytoteaseaboutsocialnormsthan comparisonparents.Recallthattheteaseswereclassi Þ ed accordingtowhethertheteasereferredtoasocialnorm violation,theindividual Õ scharacter,oranendearingnick- name.Therewerenogroupdifferencesinthenumberof timesparticipantsteasedaboutaspectsofcharacter(means appearinTableI; F (1  40)  1  82  ns ).Thenumberof teasesclassi Þ edasendearingnicknamesdidnotdiffer amongparentsandchildren, F (1  40)  1  18  ns ,nordid itdifferacrossHFAandcomparisongroups, F (1  40)  1  75  ns ,althoughchildrenintheHFAgrouptendedtouse moreendearingnicknamesthandidchildreninthecom- parisongroup, t (38)  1  80; p  10.Consistentwithour expectations,comparisongroupparticipantsteasedabout socialnormviolationsmorefrequentlythandidHFAgroup participants, F (1  40)  8  04; p  01,and,asonemight UnderstandingTeasing 63 intuit,parentsteasedaboutsocialnormviolationsmore frequentlythandidtheirchildren, F (1  40)  5  60; p   05.Morefocusedanalysesshowedthatcomparisonchil- drenteasedtheirparentsaboutsocialnormviolations morefrequentlythandidHFAchildren, t (38)  2  15; p  05.Likewise,comparisongroupparentsteasedmore frequentlythanparentsofchildrenintheHFAgroupabout socialnormviolations,thuscon Þ rmingpredictions, t (38)  2  29; p  05. Analysesoftheactualteasingbehaviorofparents andchildrenyieldedresultslargelysupportiveofourhy- potheses.Whencomparedwithappropriatecomparison individuals,childrenintheHFAgroupandtheirparents werelessplayfulandlesscriticalintheirteasing,andtheir teasingwaslesslikelytofocusonsocialnormviolations. Wenowturntoanalysesofparticipants Õ recountedexperi- encesofteasing,whichmorespeci Þ callyaddresspossible differencesintheawarenessofsocialcontext Ñ acritical partofunderstandingteasing. TeasingExperiences Weexpectedcomparisongroupparticipantstoshow agreaterawarenessofsocialcontextintheirrecounted teasingexperiences.UsingamixedmodelANOVAwith group(HFA,comparison)asthebetween-participantsfac- torandsocialcontext(antecedents,consequences)asthe within-participantsfactor,wefoundthatcomparisonchil- dren,ingeneral,referredtothesocialcontextintheir self-reportedteasingexperiencesmorefrequentlythan didchildrenwithautism, F (1  40)  12  07, p  01.As showninTableII,comparisonchildrenreferredtoan- tecedents, t (38)  2  90; p  05,andconsequencesof teasingmorefrequentlythandidHFAchildren, t (38)  TableII. TeasingConceptsandRecountedTeasingExperiences ChildrenParents HFA( N  23)Comparison( N  20)HFA( N  23)Comparison( N  20) Teasingconcepts Positive0.20(0.41)1.14(1.01)1.23(1.03)1.00(0.86) Negative2.48(1.43)2.82(0.29)3.45(2.57)1.36(0.73) SocialContestvariables Antecedent1.55(1.20)3.14(1.59)2.20(1.58)4.07(0.96) Consequences1.65(1.72)2.85(1.63)3.60(1.50)4.78(1.78) Teasingtypes Socialnorms0.10(0.30)1.64(1.79)0.48(0.55)1.54(0.88) Character0.78(1.00)0.93(0.98)1.14(0.97)1.27(1.04) Taunting0.63(0.81)0.71(0.71)0.81(0.72)0.88(0.73) Trickery0.35(0.58)0.07(0.26)0.03(0.09)0.07(0.15) Note .Meansofsocialcontextvariablesrepresenttheaveragenumberofinstancesofeachitemperrecountedteasing experience.Standarddeviationsarenotedinparentheses. 2  15; p  05.Asimilaranalysiswasconductedtoexam- ineparents Õ data.Aswithchildren,parentsinthecom- parisongroupmademorespeci Þ creferencestosocial contextinteasingexperiencesthandidparentsinthe HFAgroup, F (1  40)  21  30, p  01.Comparisonpar- entsexplicitlyreferredtobothantecedents, t (38)  3  82; p  01,andconsequences, t (38)  2  09; p  05,more frequentlythandidHFAparents. Intermsofthecategoriesofteasing,childreninboth groupsrecalled,withequivalentfrequency,teasingexpe- riencesrelatedtocharacterteasing, t (38)   60  ns ,and taunting, t (38)   59  ns ,(seeTableII).Aspredicted, childreninthecomparisongroupreportedmoreteasing experiencesrelatedtosocialnormviolationsthanHFA groupchildren, t (38)  3  01, p  01.HFAchildrenre- portedexperiencesofteasingclassi Þ edastrickerymore frequentlythancomparisongroupchildren, t (38)  2  00, p  05.Parentsofcomparisonchildrenlikewise recountedmoreinstancesofteasingaboutsocialnormvi- olationsthandidparentsofHFAchildren, t (38)  3  51, p  01.Therewerenogroupdifferencesamongparents foranyothercategoryofteasing. ConceptsofTeasing Children Our Þ nalinterestwasinthegeneralcontentofpartic- ipants Õ conceptsofteasing,elicitedwhenparticipantspro- videdde Þ nitionsofteasing.Regardlessofgroup,among children,negativeconceptsoccurredwithgreater frequency(89%)thanpositiveconcepts(11%) Ñ a Þ nding thatonewouldexpectfromthedevelopmentalliterature, whichshowsthatchildrenupthroughage11de Þ neteasing 64 Heerey,Capps,Keltner,andKring largelyinnegativeterms(Lightneretal.,2000;Warm, 1997).Childreninthecomparisongroup,however,of- feredpositiveconceptsofteasingatasigni Þ cantlygreater frequencythandidHFAgroupchildren, t (38)  3  15, p  01,consistentwithourexpectation.Thetwogroups didnotdifferintermsofthefrequencywithwhichthey de Þ nedteasinginnegativefashion, t (38)  1  12  ns (see TableII). Toascertainthatchildrenwithautismdidnotfailto expresspositiveconceptsofteasingsimplybecausethey areteasedinmorenegativewaysthancomparisonchil- dren,weexaminedthevalence(positiveornegative)of theirrecountedteasingexperiences.Recallthatrecounted teasingexperienceswerecodedasbeingeitherpositive ornegativeincontentvalence,independentofthechild Õ s emotionalevaluationoftheexperience.Thatis,itwaspos- sibleforateasetobecodedaspositiveincontentvalence (e.g.,breakingthecurveonatest),eventhoughthechild experiencedtheteaseasnegative,andviceversa.Forex- ample,onechildreportedateasethatwasnegativeincon- tentvalence(clumsilyspillingmilkatlunch)butfound hisfriends Õ teasingtobefunnyandreportedapositive experience.Codesreferringtocontentvalencearethus independentofchildren Õ sinterpretationsoftheteasing. Wefoundthatallchildrenrecalledmorenegativeteas- ingexperiences(84%)thanpositiveteasingexperiences (16%).Therewerenogroupdifferencesinrecountedteas- ingexperiencesclassi Þ edaseitherpositiveincontentva- lenceornegativeincontentvalence( positive : M control  1  10(0  62), M HFA   88(0  74), t (38)  1  36  ns ; nega- tive : M control  3  59(1  89),M HFA  4  21(1  66), t (38)   82  ns ).Thus,HFAchildrendonotappeartoofferfewer positiveconceptsofteasingsimplybecausetheyexperi- encenegativeteasingmorefrequently. Parents Liketheirchildren,parentsofHFAgrouppartici- pantsofferedmorenegativeconceptsofteasingthandid parentsofcomparisonchildren, t (38)  3  14, p  01. Interestingly,analysesrevealednogroupdifferencesin parents Õ positiveconceptsofteasing, t (38)   44  ns . LinkingParents Õ andChildren Õ sTeasing Takentogether,the Þ ndingsfromtheteasinginter- actionandinterviewsuggesttwothemes:(1)thattyp- icallydevelopingchildrenandtheirparentsappreciate thepositiveandplayfulaspectsofteasingmoresothan dohigh-functioningchildrenwithautismandAsperger Õ s syndromeandtheirparents,and(2)thattheyengagein teasingthatismorecloselylinkedtothesocialcontext thandoHFAgroupparticipants.Howmightparents Õ and children Õ steasingbehaviorsrelate?Weusedasetofcor- relationalanalysestoexplorethisquestion. Individuals Õ teasingstylesandbehaviorsduringin- teractionsarethoughttorelatetooneanother(Keltner etal.,2001).Wefoundthistobethecase,atleastamong comparisonparticipants.Criticalcontentamongchildren Õ s andparents Õ nicknameswaspositivelyassociated, r   69; p  01.Moreover,comparisonchildren Õ saf Þ liative contentwasnegativelycorrelatedwithparents Õ critical content, r   46; p  05,althoughparentswereaf- Þ liativeregardlessoftheirchildren Õ scritical, r   57; p  05,oraf Þ liativecontent, r   53; p  05.Parents Õ criticalbehaviorstendedtodecreaseaschildren Õ scriti- calteasecontentincreased, r   38; p  10.,Finally, children Õ splayfulbehaviorswerepositivelyrelatedtopar- ents Õ criticalbehaviors, r   54; p  05.Thus,among comparisondyads,participantsteasingstylesappeared related. ThestorywasmuchdifferentfortheHFAchildren andtheirparents.Correlationsbetweenparents Õ andchil- dren Õ steasingbehaviorssuggestedlittlereciprocityorin- terdependenceintheirteasing.Nosigni Þ cantcorrelation betweenHFAchildren Õ steasingbehaviorsandthoseof theirparentswerefound(seeTableIII). TheoryofMindandTeasingBehavior Might Þ ndingsrelatetochildren Õ stheoryofmind? Hierarchicalmultipleregressionanalysesaddressed whetherthegroupdifferencesinchildren Õ sreferencesto socialcontextandplayfulteasingbehaviorcouldbeac- countedforbydifferencesintheoryofmind.Weused groupasthedependentvariableineachregression.Each analysisincludedonlychildren Õ sdata,aswehadnotas- sessedparents Õ theoryofmindskills. Groupdifferencesinchildren Õ sreferencestosocial contextastheyrecountedteasingexperienceswereex- aminedaftercontrollingfortheoryofmind.Atstep1, thevariablesageandverbalIQwereentered.Asprevi- ouslyreported,nogroupdifferencesemergedasthisstep (  R 2   07; F  1  03; ns ).AtStep2,theoryofmind scoreswereenteredandweresigni Þ cantlyassociatedwith groupstatus(  R 2   18; F  6  03; p  05).Atstep3, thefrequencywithwhichparticipantsreferencedsocial contextduringteasinginteractionswasentered.Afterac- countingfortheoryofmind,teasingaboutsocialnormsno longerdifferentiatedparticipantsbygroup(  R 2   01; F   34; ns ). Theuseofplayfulnonverbalbehaviorsduringsocial interactionhasbeenrelatedtotheoryofmind UnderstandingTeasing 65 TableIII. TeasingBehaviorsAmongParentsandChildren Children Õ sbehaviors CriticalcontentAf Þ liativecontentCriticalbehaviorPlayfulbehavior HFAControlHFAControlHFAControlHFAControl Parent Õ sbehaviors Criticalcontent   09  69    04   46  .03   11.05.18 Af Þ liativecontent.14  57  .04.53    04.00   15   07 Criticalbehaviors   12   38  .23.27.09   14.05.54  Playfulbehaviors   18   07   11   27   09   25   15   20 Note .DatarepresentPearsoncorrelationsbetweenparentsandtheirchildren.  p  10.  p  05.  p  01  (Charman,1997).Asabove,inathree-stepregression,age andverbalIQdidnotdifferacrossgroups(  R 2   07; F  1  03; ns ),thoughtheoryofmindabilitydid(  R 2   18; F  6  03; p  05).Aspredicted,thegroupdiffer- enceinchildren Õ suseofplayfulbehaviorduringteasing interactionsdisappearedwhentheoryofmindwascon- trolled(  R 2   02; F   80; ns ).Differencesintheory ofmindthen,appeartoaccountfortheuseofplayfulac- tionswhileteasingandthetopicsaboutwhichchildren tease. DISCUSSION Whenhigh-functioningchildrenwithautismand theirparentsrecalledpriorepisodesofteasinganden- gagedinateasinginteraction,theytendedtoneglecttwo crucialcomponentsofteasing:(1)theplayfulbehaviors thatmitigatetheseriousnessofateaseand(2)theideathat teasingisasocialcommentaryaboutthebehaviorofan- otherindividual.Thisstudyhighlightsaninterestingpara- doxinteasing.Althoughcomparisongroupparticipants teasedinmorecriticalandmoralisticways,engagingin teasingaboutviolationsofsocialnormsmorefrequently thandidparticipantsintheHFAgroup,theirconceptsof teasingweremorepositiveandlessnegativethanthoseof HFAparticipants.Incontrast,themoreaf Þ liativeteasers inthestudy,thoseintheHFAgroup,experiencedmore dif Þ cultyseeingthepositivesideofteasing. Additionally,wefoundthatduringteasing,compar- isonparticipantsweremuchmorelikelytoshowrelat- ednessintheirteasingstyles,associatingtheirlevelsof provocationandplayfulness.These Þ ndingssuggestthat comparisongroupparticipantshadmorevariedandadapt- ableconceptsandstylesofteasing.Finally,theoryofmind accountedfordifferencesbetweencomparisonchildren Õ s andHFAchildren Õ suseofplayfulbehaviorwhenteas- ing,aswellasdifferencesinteasingaboutsocialnorms. Takentogether,these Þ ndingscorroboratepriorresearch, suggestingthattheoryofmindabilityaccountsfordiffer- encesinspontaneoussocialbehavior(e.g.,Tager-Flusberg, 1999),andtheunderstandingofsocialinteraction(e.g., Baron-Cohen&Hammer,1997). AutismandtheAbsenceofPlayinTeasing Aspredicted,HFAgroupparticipantsusedlessplay- fulbehaviorduringteasingthandidcomparisonpartici- pantsandtheoryofmindaccountedforthesedifferences. Thereareseveralpossibleaccountsofthis Þ nding.Re- latedtotheoryofminddif Þ culties,childrenwithautism andAsperger Õ ssyndromeengageinlessplay(Mundy etal.,1990).Inaddition,parentsofchildrenwithautism andchildrenwithautismthemselvesusefewernonver- balgesturesincommunication,havemoredif Þ cultypro- ducingcommunicativenonverbalgestures(Piven,Palmer, Jacobi,&Childress,1997),anddemonstrateapreference forhighlyliteralcommunicativedisplays(Baron-Cohen &Hammer,1997).Thesedifferencesintheuseofplay andnonverbalcommunicativebehaviormaygeneralize toteasing,therebyaccountingforourobservedde Þ citin playfuloff-recordmarkers. Alternately,theHFAgroupparticipantsteasedin lesscriticalfashion.Thecriticalcomponentofteasing, somewhatparadoxically,positivelycorrelateswiththein- creaseduseofoff-recordmarkers.Itmaybethecasethat HFAchildrenandtheirparentsdidnotteaseinplayful fashionbecausetheyteasedinamoreaf Þ liationfashion andthereforehadlesscriticalcontenttorenderplayful. Althoughour Þ ndingontherelationshipbetweentheory ofmindandplayfulbehaviorlendsplausibilitytothefor- merexplanation,themethodsutilizedinthepresentstudy donotallowustodistinguishconclusivelybetweenthem. AutismandtheSocial Ð MoralContentofTeasing Referencestogroupandsocialnorms,andviolations thereof:oftencomprisethemajorgrounduponwhicha 66 Heerey,Capps,Keltner,andKring teaseisconstructed(Eder,1993).TheteasingofHFA childrenandtheirparentslackedthissocialcontextual richness.Relativetocomparisonparticipants,HFAgroup participantsreferredlessfrequentlytoantecedentsand consequencesintheirrecountedexperiencesofteasing. Additionally,theirteasingcenteredlessfrequentlyonso- cialnormviolations.Thispatternofresultsisquitecon- sistentwithpreviousstudiesthathavedocumentedtheas- sociationbetweenautismandde Þ citsinsocialcontextual understandingincludingdiminishedsocial-referencing behaviors,de Þ citsinjointattention,anddecreaseduseof communicativenonverbalbehaviors(Capps&Sigman, 1996;Frith,1989).Moreover,theyhavedif Þ cultylink- ingemotionstothesocialsituationsinwhichtheyoccur (Capps,Yirmiya,&Sigman,1992),andshowde Þ cient understandingofself-consciousemotions,inwhichan- other Õ ssocialevaluationoftheselfplaysarole(Heerey, Keltner&Capps,2003). Thetendencyforchildrenwithautismtoignorethe antecedentsandconsequencesofteasing,anditsnorm- relatedfocusmoregenerally,mayhelpexplaintheanec- dotalreportsofthedif Þ cultychildrenwithautismfacein learningsocialnormsandabidingbysocialrules(Klinger &Dawson,2001).Thepresentcontext,afriendlyparent Ð childinteraction,islikelyquitedissimilartothepeerand siblingteasingmostchildrenexperience.Nonetheless,this studysuggeststhatteasingdoesnotofferchildrenwith autismanarenaforlearningsocialnorms,asitdoesfor typicallydevelopingchildren. Inthisvein,manyofthehigh-functioningchildren withautismandAsperger Õ ssyndromeinoursamplespon- taneouslyreportedthattheydidnotknowwhypeople teasedthemandoftendidnotevenunderstandthatthey werebeingteaseduntiltheybegantofeel Ò bad. Ó Whereas anumberofcomparisongroupchildrenreportedtheexpe- rienceofembarrassmentasaconsequenceofteasing,only twoHFAgroupchildrendidso.Findingsamongparents paralleledthoseoftheirchildren,suggestingthatparents ofHFAchildrenmayalsohavedif Þ cultyspontaneously linkingteasingwiththesocialcontextFinally,onthebasis ofteasinginterviews,itappearsthatHFAchildreninitiate teasinglessfrequentlyandmayhavefewersocialrelation- shipswithinwhichteasingisappropriate.Takentogether, these Þ ndingssuggestthatcomparisongroupmembers haveamorethoroughlydevelopedunderstandingofthe functionsofteasinginsocialcontextandofthewaysin whichteasingmayrelatetosocialrelationships. TeasingConcepts Conceptsofteasingarerelatedtoteasingexperi- encesandbothconceptsandexperiencesprogresswith socialandlinguisticdevelopmentAssuchdevelopment occurs,positiveconceptsofteasingbegintocoexistwith morenegativeones,allowingindividualstorecognizethe bene Þ tsofteasing.Thechildreninourstudywerejust attheagewhenthisdevelopmentalshifttypicallytakes place(Warm,1997).Accordingly,comparisonchildren evidencedbothpositiveandnegativeteasingconcepts whereastheHFAgroupchildrenhaddif Þ cultyappreciat- ingthemorepositiveaspects.Parents Õ conceptsmirrored thoseoftheirchildren.BothcomparisonandHFAgroup parentsunderstoodthatteasingcouldbepositive,how- evertheteasingconceptsofparentsofHFAchildrenwere muchmorenegative,suggestingthatalthoughtheycon- ceptuallyunderstandthatteasinghaspositiveside,they appeartointerpretitmorenegatively. CONCLUSION Inteasingoneanother,peoplelearnaboutsocial norms,roles,andexpectations.Theyexpressaffectionand explorepossiblerelationships.Theydiscovertheprefer- ences,attitudes,andbeliefsofothers.Theoryofmindand theabilitytounderstandsocialintentionsunderpinteasing andmuchofthesocialdiscourseinwhichhumansengage, anideaillustratedinthecoemergenceofearlyteasingand theoryofmindinyoungchildren(e.g.,Dunn&Munn, 1985).Our Þ ndingshintatthedif Þ cultyfacedbychil- drenwithautismandAsperger Õ ssyndromeastheytryto navigatesocialrelationshipsand Þ tintotheirsocialenvi- ronments.Unfortunately,thesedif Þ cultiesarenotunique toautismspectrumdisorders.Onthebasisoftheabili- tiesnecessarytounderstandteasingwewouldpredictthat teasingdif Þ cultieswouldextendtoanyindividualswho experiencedif Þ cultywiththeoryofmind-relatedskills, suchaschildrenwithattentionde Þ cithyperactivitydis- order(Hinshaw,1987).Becausethesedif Þ cultiesareso characteristicofchildrenwithautism,thefundamentalso- cialinteractionofteasingmayeludeeventhebestefforts ofthesechildren,despitetheirlaborstounderstandit. ACKNOWLEDGMENT WewishtothankDrDavidSobelforhiscontribution ofthetheoryofminddatasetaswellashisastutecom- mentarythroughoutthisproject.Thisworkisdedicatedto thememoryofLisaM.Capps. REFERENCES Abell,F.,Happe,F.,&Frith,U.(2000).Dotrianglesplaytricks?Attri- butionofmentalstatestoanimatedshapesinnormalandabnormal development. CognitiveDevelopment , 15, 1 Ð 16. UnderstandingTeasing 67 Bara,B.G.,Bosco,F.M.,&Bucciarelli,M.(1999).Developmental pragmaticsinnormalandabnormalchildren. BrainandLanguage , 68, 507 Ð 528. Baron-Cohen,S.,&Hammer,J.(1997).Isautismanextremeform ofthe Ò malebrain Ó ? AdvancesinInfancyResearch , 11, 193 Ð 217. Baron-Cohen,S.,O Õ Riordan,M.,Stone,V.,Jones,R.,&Plaisted,K. (1999).Recognitionoffauxpasbynormallydevelopingchildren withaspergersyndromeorhigh-functioningautism. Journalof AutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 29, 407 Ð 418. Barron,J.,&Barron,S.(1992). There Õ saboyinhere .NewYork:Simon &Schuster. Bell,R.A.,Buerkel-Rothfuss,N.L.,&Gore,K.E.(1987). Ò Didyou bringtheyarmulkeforthecabbagepatchkid? Ó Theidiomaticcom- municationofyounglovers. HumanCommunicationResearch , 14, 47 Ð 67. Bell,R.A.,&Healey,J.G.(1992).Idiomaticcommunicationandinter- personalsolidarityinfriends Õ relationalcultures. HumanCommu- nicationResearch , 18, 307 Ð 335. Capps,L.,&Sigman,M.(1996).Autisticaloneness.InR.D.Kavanaugh, B.Zimmerberg,etal.(Eds.), Emotion:Interdisciplinaryperspec- tives (pp.273 Ð 296).Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum. Capps,L.,Yirmiya,N.,&Sigman,M.(1992).Understandingofsimple andcomplexemotionsinnon-retardedchildrenwithautism. Jour- nalofChildPsychologyandPsychiatryandAlliedDisciplines , 33, 1169 Ð 1182. Charman,T.(1997).Therelationshipbetweenjointattentionandpretend playinautism. DevelopmentandPsychopathology , 9, 1 Ð 16. Charman,T.,&Baron-Cohen,S.(1997).Briefreport:Promptedpretend playinautism. JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 27, 325 Ð 332. Clark,H.H.(1996).Communities,rommonalities,andcommunica- tion.InJ.J.Gumperz&S.C.Levinson(Eds.), Rethinkinglin- guisticrelativity (pp.324 Ð 355).NewYork:CambridgeUniversity Press. Clark,H.H.,&Gerrig,R.J.(1984).Onthepretensetheoryofirony. JournalofExperimentalPsychology:General , 113, 121 Ð 126. Dunn,J.F.,&Munn,P.(1985).Becomingafamilymember:Family con ß ictandthedevelopmentofsocialunderstandinginthesecond year. ChildDevelopment , 56, 480 Ð 492. Eder,D.(1993). Ò GogetyaaFrench! Ó :Romanticandsexualteasing amongadolescentgirls.InD.Tannen(Ed.), Genderandconver- sationalinteraction (pp.17 Ð 31).NewYork:OxfordUniversity Press. Eisenberg,A.R.(1986).Teasing:VerbalplayintwoMexicanohomes.In B.B.Schieffelin&E.Ochs&etal.(Eds.), Languagesocialization acrosscultures (pp.182 Ð 198).NewYork:CambridgeUniversity Press. Flavell,J.H.(1999).Cognitivedevelopment:Children Õ sknowledge aboutthemind. AnnualReviewofPsychology , 50, 21 Ð 45. Frith,U.(1989).Autismand Ò theoryofmind. Ó InC.Gillberg(Ed.), Diagnosisandtreatmentofautism (pp.33 Ð 52).NewYork:Plenum. Frith,U.,Happe,F.,&Siddons,F.(1994).Autismandtheoryofmind ineverydaylife. SocialDevelopment , 3, 108 Ð 124. Glass,G.V.,&Hopkins,K.D.(1996). Statisticalmethodsineduca- tionandpsychology (3rded.).NeedhamHeights,MA:Allyn& Bacon. Grandin,T.(1995).Howpeoplewithautismthink.InE.Schopler&G.B. Mesibov(Eds.), Learningandcognitioninautism .(pp.137 Ð 156). NewYork:Plenum. Happe,F.G.E.(1993).Communicativecompetenceandtheoryofmind inautism:Atestofrelevancetheory. Cognition , 48, 101 Ð 119. Happe,F.G.E.(1994).Anadvancedtestoftheoryofmind:Under- standingofstorycharacters Õ thoughtsandfeelingsbyableautistic, mentallyhandicapped,andnormalchildrenandadults. Journalof AutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 24, 129 Ð 154. Heerey,E.A.,Keltner,D.,andCapps,L.M.(2003).Makingsenseof self-consciousemotion:Linkingtheoryofmindandemotionin childrenwithautism, Emotion , 3, 394 Ð 400. Hinshaw,S.P.(1987).Onthedistinctionbetweenattentional de Þ cits/hyperactivityandconductproblems/aggressionin childpsychopathology. PsychologicalBulletin , 101, 443 Ð 463. Hobson,J.A.(Ed.).(1989). Abnormalstatesofbrainandmind . Cambridge,MA:Birkhaeuser. Jarrold,C.,Boucher,J.,&Smith,P.(1993).Symbolicplayinautism:A review. JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 23, 281 Ð 307. Keltner,D.,Capps,L.,Kring,A.M.,Young,R.C.,&Heerey,E.A. (2001).Justteasing:Aconceptualanalysisandempiricalreview. PsychologicalBulletin , 127, 229 Ð 248. Keltner,D.,Young,R.C.,Heerey,E.A.,Oemig,C.,&Monarch,N.D. (1998).Teasinginhierarchicalandintimaterelations. Journalof PersonalitvandSocialPsychology , 75, 1231 Ð 1247. Klinger;L.G.,&Dawson,G.(2001).Prototypefonnationinautism. DevelopmentandPsychopathology , 13, 111 Ð 124. Leslie,A.M.(1987).Pretenseandrepresentation:Theoriginsof Ò theory ofmind. Ó PsychologicalReview , 94, 412 Ð 426. Lightner,R.M.,Bollmer,J.M.,Harris,M.J.,Milich,R.,&Scambler, D.J.(2000).Whatdoyousaytoteasers?Parentandchildevalu- ationsofresponsestoteasing. JournalofAppliedDevelopmental Psychology , 21, 403 Ð 427. Lord,C.,Rutter,M.,&LeCouteur,A.(1994).AutismDiagnostic Interview-Revised:Arevisedversionofadiagnosticinterviewfor caregiversofindividualswithpossiblepervasiyedevelopmentaldis- orders. JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 24, 659 Ð 685. Loveland,K.A.,Pearson,D.A.,Tunali-Kotoski,B.,Ortegon,J.,& Gibbs,M.C.(2001).Judgementsofsocialappropriatenessbychil- drenandadolescentswithautism. JournalofAutism&Develop- mentalDisorders , 31, 367 Ð 376. McDonnell,J.T.(1993). Newsfromtheborder:Amother Õ smemoirof herautisticson .NewYork:TicknorandFields. Mundy,P.,Sigman,M.,&Kasari,C.(1990).Alongitudinalstudyofjoint attentionandlanguagedevelopmentinautisticchildren. Journalof AutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 20, 115 Ð 128. Ozonoff,S.,Rogers,S.J.,&Pennington,B.F.(1991).Asperger Õ ssyn- drome:Evidenceofanempiricaldistinctionfromhigh-functioning autism. JournalofChildPsychologyandPsychiatryandAllied Disciplines , 32, 1107 Ð 1122. Perner,J.,Frith,U.,Leslie,A.M.,&Leekam,S.R.(1989).Exploration oftheautisticchild Õ stheoryofmind:Knowledge,belief,andcom- munication. ChildDevelopment , 60, 689 Ð 700 Pilowsky,T.,Yirmiya,N.,Shulman,C.,&Dover,R.(1998).TheAutism DiagnosticInterview-RevisedandtheChildhoodAutismRating Scale:Differencesbetweendiagnosticsystemandcomparisonbe- tweengenderes. JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 28, 143 Ð 151. Piven,J.,Palmer,P.,Jacobi,D.,&Childress,D.(1997).Broaderautism phenotype:Evidencefromafamilyhistorystudyofmultiple- incidenceautismfamilies. AmericanJournalofPsychiatry , 154, 185 Ð 190. Shapiro,J.P.,Baumeister,R.F.,&Kessler,J.W.(1991).Athree- componentmodelofchildren Õ steasing:Aggression,humor,and ambiguity. JournalofSocialandClinicalPsychology , 10, 459 Ð 472. Siegel,B.(1986).Empiricallyderivedsubclassi Þ cationoftheautistic syndrome. JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders , 16, 275 Ð 293. Smith,P.K.,&Brain,P.(2000).Bullyinginschools:Lessonsfromtwo decadesofresearch. AggressiveBehavior , 26, 1 Ð 9. Sobel,D.M.,Capps,L.M.,&Gopnik,A.(1999,May).Explor- ingtherelationshipbetweenautisticchildren Õ sperceptionofam- biguous Þ guresandtheoryofmind.Paperpresentedatthean- nualmeetingoftheSocietyforResearchinChildDevelopment, Denver,CO. Strandburg,R.J.,Marsh,J.T.,Brown,W.S.,&Asamow,R.F. (1993).Event-relatedpotentialsinhigh-functioningadultautistics: 68 Heerey,Capps,Keltner,andKring Linguisticandnonlinguisticvisualinformationprocessingtasks. Neuropsychologia , 31, 413 Ð 434. Szatmari,P.,Offord,D.R.,Siegel,L.S.,&Finlayson,M.A.(1990).The clinicalsigni Þ canceofneurocognitiveimpairmentsamongchildren withpsychiatricdisorders:Diagnosisandsituationalspeci Þ city. JournalofChildPsychologyandPsychiatryandAlliedDisciplines , 31, 287 Ð 299. Tager-Flusberg,H.(1999).Apsychologicalapproachtounderstanding thesocialandlanguageimpairmentsinautism. InternationalRe- viewofPsychiatry , 11, 325 Ð 334. Warm,T.R.(1997).Theroleofteasingindevelopmentandviceversa. JournalofDevelopmentalandBehavioralPediatrics , 18, 97 Ð 101. Wechsler,D.(1991). WechslerIntelligenceScaleforChildren (3rded.). SanAntonio,TX:PsychologicalCorporation.