/
Why Do We Gesture When We Speak?Robert M. KraussColumbia Universitydo Why Do We Gesture When We Speak?Robert M. KraussColumbia Universitydo

Why Do We Gesture When We Speak?Robert M. KraussColumbia Universitydo - PDF document

tatiana-dople
tatiana-dople . @tatiana-dople
Follow
373 views
Uploaded On 2016-12-25

Why Do We Gesture When We Speak?Robert M. KraussColumbia Universitydo - PPT Presentation

relationship of gesture and language in the form of a simple analogy As thecommunication is not the only function such gestures serve Over the pastgestures help speakers formulate coherent speech ID: 505652

relationship gesture and language

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Pdf The PPT/PDF document "Why Do We Gesture When We Speak?Robert M..." is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

Why Do We Gesture When We Speak?Robert M. KraussColumbia Universitydo we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science 7, 54-59. relationship of gesture and language in the form of a simple analogy: "As thecommunication is not the only function such gestures serve. Over the pastgestures help speakers formulate coherent speech by aiding in the retrieval ofHow might gesturing affect lexical retrieval? Human memory employs University, 1190 Amsterdam Ave., Mail Code 5501, New York, NY 10027. E-mail to:observer at least, appear related to the semantic content of the speech they accompany). It isthe latter type that is the focus of the work reported here. Conversational gestures should bedistinguished from the stereotyped hand configurations and movements with specific,See Kendon (1994) and Krauss, Chawla & Chen (1996) for contrasting views. memory is multiply encoded in more than one representational format. When aspeech; (2) the temporal relation of speech and gesture; (3) the influence ofbeen memorized contains many fewer pauses, and nearly all of them arethem, and more nonjuncture pauses, in spontaneous speech than in rehearsedgiven to another actor of the same sex, who was asked to portray the original temporal relationship to the speech they are presumed to facilitate. Forword if the gesture were initiated after the word had been articulated. The wordit) for 60 lexical gestures drawn from a corpus of speakers describing a variety of The durations of lexical gestures vary considerably. The average length ofbriefest was 0.54 s, and the longest 7.71 s. We hypothesized that a gesture'sbefore the lexical gesture terminated. Note that all but three of the 60 data content of what is being said, and we should observe an association betweengesturing and conceptual content. Flora Fan Zhang, who at the time the studyproject. She videotaped speakers as they defined twenty common English(Figure 3). The words differ on several dimensions,nonspatial). All three are correlated with the proportion of time the speakerHowever most of variance is attributable to the Spatiality factor. The simple those phrases. We then did the same for the remaining two-thirds of the phrasesincreased the difficulty of retrieval by asking subjects to use uncommon words Th from their palms. conceptualizing, formulating,the formulating stage, the preverbal message is transformed in two ways. First,source of corrective feedback.of the process. When this happens, the speaker may speak more slowly, pausea word, restart the sentence, etc. We expected that preventing speakers from models have been proposed that differ in significant ways. For present purposes the differencesare less important than the similarities. The account we give is based on Levelt (1989), but allof the models with which we are familiar make similar distinctions. content phrases and elsewhere. The normal, obscure and constrained speechSpeakers spoke more slowly in the obscure and constrained speech conditionsrespectively). They also spoke more slowly when they were not permitted tominute, across the three speech conditions). With other kinds of content,speakers spoke somewhat more rapidly when they could not gesture. Theelsewhere. Not surprisingly, speakers were more dysfluent overall in thethey are considerably more dysfluent during SCPs than elsewhere. As Figure 4 used measure of lexical diversity. Both indices are related to accessibility. nonjuncture filled pauses. Preventing speakers from gesturing had the sameclause boundaries. When they could not gesture, that percentage increased to See (Krauss et al., 1996) for a review of evidence supporting this claim. problems in word finding, these results indicate that preventing speakers fromaffecting the ease or difficulty of retrieving words from lexical memory. What isretrieval of the word form or lexeme. All three could result in slow andprimary effect of gesturing is on retrieval of the word form. The argument isgesturing in patients who have suffered brain damage due to strokes. Patientspress) . Anomic patients have relatively good comprehension and can repeatwords and short phrases, but do poorly in tests of object naming. Moreover, definitions of uncommon words and try to recall the word form. The definitionscold winter day. One man chattered away animatedly, while other nodded fromreplied Shmuel, "I forgot my gloves." At the time, I didn't see the point of thestory. Half a century later it has become a primary focus of my research. CD.3 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16Frequency Gesture-Speech Asynchrony (s) Distribution of Gesture-Speech Asynchronies (onset time of speech minus onset CD.3 Duration (s)-1Asynchrony (s) Figure 2Duration of lexical gestures plotted against speech-gesture asynchrony (both ins). The heavier line is the unit line; the lighter line above it is the least-squaresregression line (see text for explanation). CD.3 0.10.20.30.40.5 CD.3 Spatial ContentOther Content No Gesture Gesture Figure 4Dysfluency rates (number of long and short pauses, filled pauses, incompletedand repeated words, and restarted sentences per word) in gesture and nogesture conditions for spatial and nonspatial content CD.3 Nonjuncture Filled Pauses No Gesture Conditional probability of nonjuncture filled pause (Pr (NonJ FP |FP) in CD.3Bacon, F. (1891). The advancement of learning, Book 230Frick-Horbury, D., & Guttentag, R. E. (in press). The effects of restricting handgesture production on lexical retrieval and free recall. gestures and speech: A neurolinguistic investigation. Cognitive Processes.Hadar, U., Wenkert-Olenik, D., Krauss, R. M., & Soroker, N. (in press). Gestureand the processing of speech: Neuropsychological evidence. Kendon, A. (1994). Do gestures communicate?: A review. Research on Languageand Social Interaction 27Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Chawla, P. (1996). Nonverbal behavior andnonverbal communication: What do conversational hand gestures tell us?In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 389-450).and action . There has been a change of publisher for the volume. It willbe listed as in press as soon as a new publisher is selected.]Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulationThe MIT Press.Morrel-Samuels, P., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Word familiarity predicts temporalasynchrony of hand gestures and speech. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition18, 226-231. and Flora Fan Zhang were my collaborators in the studies described here. Theand SBR 93-10586. I've benefited greatly from discussions of these matters with