/
Matingconflictinprimates:infanticide,sexualharassmentandfemalesexualit Matingconflictinprimates:infanticide,sexualharassmentandfemalesexualit

Matingconflictinprimates:infanticide,sexualharassmentandfemalesexualit - PDF document

tawny-fly
tawny-fly . @tawny-fly
Follow
362 views
Uploaded On 2016-04-28

Matingconflictinprimates:infanticide,sexualharassmentandfemalesexualit - PPT Presentation

141 142 Secondweturntosexualharassmentiemaleaggressiontargetedagainstsexuallyactive ID: 296421

141 142 Second weturntosexualharassment i.e.maleaggressiontargetedagainstsexuallyactive(

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Pdf The PPT/PDF document "Matingconflictinprimates:infanticide,sex..." is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

141 Matingconflictinprimates:infanticide,sexualharassmentandfemalesexualityCARELP.VANSCHAIK,GAURIR.PRADHAN&MARIAA.VANNOORDWIJKINTRODUCTIONInavarietyofmammalsandafewbirds,newlyimmigratedornewlydominantmalesareknowntoattackandkilldependentinfants(Hausfater&Hrdy,1984;Parmigiani&vomSaal,1994;vanSchaik&Janson,2000).Hrdy(1974)wasthefirsttosuggest 142 Second,weturntosexualharassment,i.e.maleaggressiontargetedagainstsexuallyactive(‘oestrous’)females,andshowhowitcanbeviewedasanexpressionofmatingconflictbetweenthefemaleandthedominantmale.Becauseharassmentisremarkablyconcentratedincatarrhineprimates,wethenexaminesexualbehaviourandphysiologyinthatradiationrelativetospeciesvulnerabletoinfanticideinotherprimateradiations.Weconcludethatvariousfeaturesofcatarrhinesexualitycanplausiblybeunderstoodasresponsestothisharassmentintheevolutionaryarmsrace,althoughthistopicrequiresmuchadditionalwork.MalesasprotectorsBeforediscussingaspectsoffemalesexualbehaviourasacounterstrategyagainstinfanticidesomeattentiontotheassociationwithlikelysiresisneededinordertoexplainwhyfemalesexualbehaviourtendstoleadtoprotectionbyone,orsometimesmore,likelysires.Protectionofinfantsbylikelysiresismadepossiblebytheyear-roundmale-femaleassociationfoundinvirtuallyallprimatesinwhichfemalescarrytheirinfants(vanSchaik&Kappeler,1997).Atleastinmulti-malegroups,likelysirestendtobeinclosespatialproximitywithinfants(Janson,1986;Pauletal.,2000),andnumerousreportsindicatethatthesemalesactuallydefendtheinfantsagainstattacksbyothermales(Borriesetal.,1999;review:vanSchaik,2000a).Infanticideoftenhappenswhentheformerdominantmale,themostlikelysireofmostinfantseveninmultimalegroups(reviewsinCowlishaw&Dunbar,1991;Paul,1997;vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,thisvolume),iseliminatedorincapacitated.Infanticidalattackscouldhavebeenprovokedbyhisexperimentalremoval,eitherinthewild(Sugiyama,1966)or(quitefrequently,andinadvertently)in Wefollowtheuseof"harassment"asinSmuts&Smuts(1993),whichdiffersfromthatinClutton-Brock&Parker(1995).captivity(e.g.,Angst&Thommen,1977),orbynaturaldemographicprocesses(e.g.,Steenbeek,1996).Inallthesesituationsinfanticidebythenewmaleiscommonlyobserved.Conversely,whenoneinterpretsthesocialcontextofalldirectlyobservedcasesofinfanticideinwildprimatesthatoccurredspontaneously,thegreatmajority(85%of55cases:vanSchaik,2000a)isassociatedwithachangeinthedominantpositioninthegroup,whichinvolvestheoustingorweakeningoftheformerdominant.Relativetakeoverrate(correctedforvariationininterbirthinterval)alsoexplainsmuchofthevariationininfanticideratesinwell-studiedpopulations(Janson&vanSchaik,2000).Thus,dominantmalesareeffectiveprotectorsofinfantsaslongastheyarenotoustedorincapacitated.Femalesshouldthereforebeexpectedtomatepreferentiallywiththesepowerfulmaleswheneverfemalescanbeconfrontedbymaleswhoareunlikelysires.FEMALESEXUALBEHAVIOURASACOUNTER-STRATEGYTOINFANTICIDETheoryHrdy(1974,1979)hypothesizedthatfemalesexualbehaviourinprimatespeciesvulnerabletoinfanticidehasbeenmodifiedtoreducetheriskofinfanticidebymales.Thebasisfortheargumentisthatprimatemales,likethoseofthemajorityofmammals,donotrecognisetheiroffspring(initselfpossiblytheproductoffemale-drivenevolution),andthereforemustrelyonindirectindicatorsofpaternityprobability.Theseindicatorshavebeenstudiedexperimentallyinrodents(vomSaal,1984;Perrigo&vomSaal,1994),butnotinprimates,wheretheindicatorshavetobepiecedtogetherbyanalysingindividualcases(reviewedinvanSchaik,2000a).Primatemalesarethoughttouserulesofthumbsuchasthequalityoftheirsexualexperiencewiththefemale(i.e.matingfrequencyrelativetoher 143 attractivity[her‘stimulusvalue’tothemale])weightedforhermatingfrequencywithothermales,theintervalbetweenmatingsandbirth,andperhapsthecontinuityofassociationbetweenmaleandfemale.Weassumethatnaturalselectionhasfavouredmalesthatusethoserulesofthumbthatyieldtheclosestaveragematchwiththeprobabilityofpaternity.Conceptually,wecandistinguishtwokindsofmatingsbyfemalesthatmayreducetheriskofinfanticide.First,bymatingpolyandrouslyinpotentiallyfertileperiods,femalescanreducetheconcentrationofpaternityinthedominantmale,andspreadsomeofittoothermales,sothatlong-termaveragepaternityprobabilitieswillbesomewhatbelow1forthedominantandsomewhatabove0forthesubordinates.Second,bymatingduringperiodsofnon-fertility(e.g.duringpregnancy;situation-dependentreceptivity:Hrdy,1979),afemalemaybeabletomanipulatetheassessmentbythevariousmalesoftheirpaternitychances,althoughsheobviouslycannotchangetheactualpaternityvaluesallocatedtothevariousmales.Thisdistinctionistheoreticallyuseful,butthevariousplayersareprobablyquiteunawareofit,withmalesmerelyrespondingtovisual,olfactoryandbehaviouralstimuliemanatingfromthefemalethatcreatevariationinherattractivity(Snowdon,thisvolume;Zinneretal.,thisvolume).Ifmalebehaviourdependsontheirestimateofpaternity,thissexualbehaviourcanbeeffective.Hrdy(1979,1997)reasonedthatwheremaleshavealow,butnon-zeroprobability,theywouldrefrainfromattackingtheinfant,whereastheywoulddefenditwhentheestimateishigher.EmpiricalevidenceThebasicpredictionisthatfemalesthatarevulnerabletoinfanticidebymalesshouldbeactivelypolyandrouswheneverpotentiallyinfanticidalmalesarepresentinthematingpool(i.e.thesexuallymaturemalesinthesocialunitornearbywithwhomthefemalecanmateinprinciple).Thereisampleevidencethatprimatefemalesinvulnerablespeciesactivelypursuepolyandrousmatingsandthattheyoftenengageinmatingswhenfertilizationisunlikelyorimpossible(Small,1993;Manson,1995;summarisedinvanSchaiketal.,1999).Indeed,femalesoftentargetlow-rankingorperipheralmalesreluctanttomateinthepresenceofdominantcentralmales,especiallyduringpregnancy(e.g.,Watts,1991;Wallis&Bettinger,1993;Gust,1994).Therearetwosourcesofmoredirectempiricalevidencetoassesswhetherthesederivedfeaturesofprimatesexualbehaviourareindeedanevolutionaryresponsetovulnerabilitytoinfanticide:(i)directsexualresponsesbyfemalestochangesingroupcompositionormalestatus;and(ii)broadercomparisonsofsexualbehaviourbetweentaxathatareorarenotvulnerabletoinfanticidebymales.Inspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticide,femalesoftenrespondtochangesinthemalecohortofagroupwithimmediateproceptivityandeffectivelysolicitmatingswiththenew(-lydominant)male(Struhsaker&Leland,1985;Cords,1988;Sommeretal.,1992;Swedell,2000),evenshowingrapiddevelopmentofsexualswellingsinspeciesthathavethem(Stein,1984;Colmenares&Gomendio,1988;Zinner&Deschner,2000).Invariouscatarrhineprimatesinwhichmultiplemalestemporarilyenteragroup(‘maleinflux’),matingperiods(durationofoestrus)arerelativelylongercomparedtoperiodswithoutmaleinfluxes(e.g.,Cords,1984,1988;Takahataal.,1994).Similarly,inseveralspecieswithbothsinglemaleandmulti-malegroups,femalematingperiodsarelongerinmulti-malegroups(Brockman,1999;Heistermannetal.,2001).Interspecificcomparisonsprovidesimilarsupport:vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik(2000)foundacleartrendtowardmorepolyandrousmatingamongprimateandcarnivorespeciesvulnerabletoinfanticiderelativetothosethatarenotvulnerable.Post-conceptionmating,whileinfrequentlyreported,wasalsoconcentratedinthoseordersofmammals 144 whereinfanticideistobeexpectedbasedontheirlifehistory.Withinprimates,post-conceptionmatingsarefoundpredominantly,andperhapsexclusively,inspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticide(vanSchaiketal.1999).However,onepredictionwasnotupheld:inmostothermammalsnosystematictrendtowardslongermatingperiodsinspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticidewasapparent(vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,2000).Wewilllatershowthatthispredictionisonlyexpectedwheremalesareabletoforcematings.Adifferentsourceofevidencefortheeffectivenessofsexualbehaviourinreducingtheriskofinfanticideisthelowerrateofinfanticideinmulti-malegroups,whencontrollingfortheeffectoftakeoverofdominance(Janson&vanSchaik,2000).Tosomeextentthisreductionisobviouslyduetomaleprotectionbecauseinmulti-malegroupsdefeateddominantstendtoremaininthegroup,atleastforawhile(e.g.,vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,1988;Perry,1998;Borries,2000).However,sexualstrategiesareimplicatedaswellbecauseweoccasionallyseeprotectionoftheinfantbyotherresidentmales(e.g.,Borriesetal.,1999),orabsenceofattacksbythenewdominantwhowasalong-termresidentandhadmatedbeforewiththemothers(seebelow).AproblemAremarkableaspectofinfanticideisthat–especiallyinmulti-malegroups–onlyasmalltomoderateproportionoftheinfantstypicallyendsupgettingkilled.Whilethishighprobabilityofsurvivalprobablyhasmultiplesources,itisreasonabletoattributeatleastsomeofittothefemale’ssexualbehaviour.Yet,thelatter’seffectivenessissomewhatsurprisingbecausepaternityisaconstant-sumgame,inthateachinfantcanhaveonlyonesireandthatthelong-termaverageprobabilitiesoffertilisationofalltheplayersmustaddupto1.Afemalethereforefacesaconsiderablechallenge.Ontheonehand,byraisingthelong-termprobabilityofsubordinatemalesthroughtheirmatingbehaviour,shereducestheriskofinfanticidebecausethesemalesarelesslikelytoattacktheinfantwhentheybecomedominant.Ontheotherhand,thisbehaviourmustreducethepaternityprobabilityofthedominantmale,andhencemakeitlesslikelythatthedominantwilldefendtheinfantagainstmalesenteringfromtheoutside(cf.Symons,1982).Itisthereforenotimmediatelyobviousthatsexualbehaviourcouldeverachieveanoptimalbalanceandreduceoverallrisk.Here,wewillpresentanovelexplanationfortheeffectivenessofsexualbehaviour.Doingso,however,requiresthatwefirstdeterminetheconditionsinwhichnaturalselectionfavoursinfanticidebymales(cf.vanSchaik,2000a).Whenisinfanticidefavouredbynaturalselection?Inaspeciesinwhichinfanticideadvancesthefemale’snextconception,andinasituationinwhichamalecanbeconfidentthat (i.e.henevermatedwiththefemale,where istheprobabilityofhavingsiredtheexistinginfant),infanticideisobviouslyanadvantageousstrategy,provideditcanbecommittedatlowcost.However,ifthemalehasamatinghistorywiththefemale,amorequantitativepredictionisneeded.Inordertodevelopthisprediction,wecomparetheexpectedmeannumberofoffspringsiredbyadominantmaleduringhisperiodofdominance(tenure)undertwoscenarios:withandwithoutinfanticideuponassumingthedominantposition.Denotetheeffectivetenureperiodofanon-infanticidaldominantmaleas ,i.e.theperiodbetweentheconceptionofthefirstinfantsiredduringthenewtenuretotheendofthemale’stenure.Iftheregularinterbirthintervalis ,andtheinterbirthintervalfollowinginfanticideis ,thetimegainedbyaninfanticidalmaleis.Thus,theeffectivetenureofaninfanticidalmaleis.Nowwecancalculatethebenefitofthetwostrategies( 145 and,fornon-infanticideandinfanticide,respectively).Thesebenefitsintermsofexpectednumberofinfantsare(vanSchaik[2000a],whichoverlookedinfantssiredbynon-infanticidalmalesbeforebecomingdominant): BpP T i B p P =Š+where istheprobabilityofsiringinfantsduringtenure,assumingthatthisprobabilityisconstantregardlessofwhetherthemalecommitsinfanticide.Then,thenetbenefitofcommittinginfanticideis: ttT BpPp Š=Š+ŠŠ Thusispositive(giventhat)if Inequality(1)ignoresanycoststoinfanticide(seevanSchaik[2000a]fordiscussion).Intheaverageprimatespecies,themaximum isaround0.5,whichisattainedwhenanewborniskilled.Inthiscase.Astheinfantgetsolder, hastoincreasetomakeinfanticideadvantageoustothemale.Fortheobservedmeanvaluesof between0.25to0.32(seeabove),infanticideisadvantageousif isgreaterthanapproximately OptimummaledecisionsInequality(1)showswheninfanticideisexpected,butwecannotassumethatmaleshaveperfectestimatesoftherelevantparameters.Wemustthereforetranslatethesecriteriaintodecision-makingrulesformales.Somecasesaresimple.First,ifthenewlydominantmalenevermatedwiththefemale,and,theruleiseasy.Indeed,infanticideiscommonlyseenafteranewmaleimmigranttakesoveragroup(e.g.,Steenbeek,1996;vanSchaik,2000a).Second,ifthemalehasasexualhistorywiththefemalebuthisestimateof isverysmall,infantagemattersbecausethebenefitsdecreaseasinfantsgetolder(cf.Crockett&Sekulic,1984;Sommer,1994;summarisedinFig.2.1invanSchaik,2000a).Inmulti-malegroups,however,amalehasgenerallymatedwiththefemalebefore,andoptimumdecisionmakingmaybemoredifficult.Themaleisforcedtouseindirectindicatorsbasedonhissexualhistorywiththefemale(seeabove)toproduceestimatesthatwilltendtobehighlyimprecise,exceptwhenthemalecouldmonopolisemostmatingswithsexuallyhighlyattractivefemalesorwhenhecouldgetonlyveryfewmatingswhenshewasnotveryattractive.Frequentpolyandrousmatingduringbothperiodsofovarianactivityandperiodsofpatentinfertility(e.g.pregnancy)couldhavetwoconsequences.First,itmayleadtoanincreaseintheestimatedpaternityprobability(henceforth ),especiallyifthemalesarenotfullyinformedaboutthefemalematingactivitywithothermales.Thus,especiallybymatingfrequentlyattimesofnon-fertility,femalesmaymanagetoincrease ofespeciallylow-rankingmales,producingasumoftheseestimatesgreaterthan1(obviously,theactualpaternityprobabilitiesstilladdupto1).Second,frequentpolyandryshouldleadtogreatuncertaintyofeachmale’s .Onemustassumethathigherquantityofmatingscancompensatetosomeextentforlowerquality(i.e.thefemalewaslessattractive).Wesuspectthatnon-dominantmaleshaveveryimpreciseestimatesoftheirchancesofpaternity.Inasituationofhighuncertaintyastothevalueof itmaybeimpossibletofindoptimumdecision-makingalgorithms. 146 Table8.1.Thepayoffsofdecisionsmadebynewlydominantmaleswithrespecttoinfants,dependingonwhetherthemalehadsiredthisinfantornot.textforexplicationofvariables Decision:FatherNotfatherKill T i P tn i P tn Notkill T P t n + T P t n Anewlydominantmalemaydecidetochoosetheoptionthatmaximizesmeanfitness.Ifuncertaintyoverthevalueof approachesignorance,hisbestguessmaybethat ,anditiseasytoshowthatnotkillingtheinfantisonaveragethebestoption(seeTable8.1).However,sinceagivenmaleactuallydidordidnotfathertheinfant,inacaselikethis,anewlydominantmalemayactuallymaximisehisfitnesspayoffbyavoidingcostlymistakes(cf.Resnik,1987,p.28),i.e.minimizetheriskoflosingalargeportionoffitness(cf.riskavoidanceinforaging:Stephens&Krebs,1986).Anewlydominantmalecanmaketwokindsofmistakes:(i)hecankillaninfantthathehadactuallysiredbeforebecomingdominant,thuslosingoneinfantfromhistotalnumberproduced;and(ii)hecanrefrainfromkillinganinfantthathedidnotsire,thuslosingtimetothenextconceptionoftheinfant’smother.Thesetwoerrorshavedifferentcostsattachedtothem.Table8.1presentsthepayoffsofthetwopossiblemaledecisions(killvsnottokill),undertwodifferentconditions(maleactuallyfatheredtheinfantvsdidnot).Thecostofthemistake,i.e.themalekillinghisowninfant,relativetotheoptimumtacticofrefrainingfromkillingit,isthedifferencebetweenkillingitandnotkillingit: 111Ttt P t =Š+ŠŠ t t P =Š+Thusthecostofkillinghisowninfantisintherange 211 ŠŠ(theseareininfantunits).Ontheotherhand,ifthemaledidnotsiretheinfant,thecostofnotkillingtheinfant(relativetotheoptimumtacticofkillingit)is:Thecostofnotkillinganothermale’sinfantisintherange 120. ŠCommittingthefirsterror(killingone’sowninfant)hasfargreatercosts( 1 C )thancommittingtheseconderror(notkillingsomeothermale’sinfant; ).Hence,iffemalematingtacticshaveconfusedpaternityestimatestothepointofnear-ignorance,anewlydominantmalewilldobettertoavoidthemorecostlyerror,andshouldthusrefrainfrominfanticide.Thiseffectofdeceptivefemalematingscouldexplainwhyinfanticideisnotalwaysseeninconditionswhereitmightbeexpected.Weassumethatthedominantmalesusuallyhavelessuncertaintyconcerningtheirdecisionwhethertoprotectvsnottoprotectaninfant,becausetheirestimatesofpaternitywilltendtobecloseto1.However,iftheyalsofaceconsiderableuncertaintyvergingonignorance,theyshouldalsoavoidmakingthecostliermistake.Table8.2providesthepayoffsofthedominantmale’sdecisionsforthetwopossiblestates(fathervsnon-father).Thecostsofnotprotectinganinfantthatthemaleactuallysiredis:3()2 sscs =ŠŠŠ=Š+(where=probabilityofinfantsurvival;and=costofprotection,bothexpressedininfantunits),whereasthecostofprotectinganinfantthathedidnotsireis: = ŠPtTn PtttninŠŠ= PtttTninŠ+ 147 Table8.2.Thepayoffsofdecisionsmadebycurrentlydominantmaleswithrespecttoinfantsdependingonwhetherthemalehadsiredthisinfantornot. Decision:FatherNotfatherProtect c Š Notprotect 0 c =ŠŠ=ŠFor (averyreasonableassumption),thedecisionnottoprotecttheinfantwhenthedominantmaleistheactualsireisthecostlierone.Thus,ifadominantmaleissouncertainofhispaternityastobevirtuallyignorantofitsvalue(i.e.,ifhisestimateiscloseto0.5),thenhisbestdecisionistoprotecttheinfantwhenitisatrisk,uptoapoint.Italsosuggests,however,thatmalesfacinghighercostsofprotection,i.e.lesspowerfulorinjuredmales,arelesslikelytoprotectinfants,ortoprotectwithlesserintensity,eveniftheyhavematedextensivelywiththemother.Asinthecaseofthenewlydominantmale,thisapproachleadstothesameconclusionastheonethatmaximiseshismeanfitness(assumingthat ConclusionsfromthemaledecisionmodelThemainconclusionfromthisanalysisisthatfemalepolyandryandmatingduringnon-fertileperiodsservetoraisetheestimatedpaternityprobability( valuesforallmalesinvolved,andmakingtheirsumexceed1.Polyandrymayalsoconfuse tothepointthatthemales’bestcourseofactionistorefrainfromkillinginfantsandtoprotectthemiftheyhavematedextensively,eveniftheyactuallydidnotsirethem.Thus,femalesexualbehaviourmayservetoovercometheconstant-sumnatureofthepaternitygame.Unfortunately,developingconvincingtestsoftheseideasisnoteasy,especiallybecauseincompleteinfanticidalattacksbynewlydominantmalesandthereducedratesofinfanticideinmulti-malespeciescanhavemultiplesources,asnotedabove.Nonetheless,webelieveitismeaningfultotranslatetheconditionsinwhichnaturalselectionshouldfavouroneactionoveranotherintotheactualdecision-makingprocessesofanimals.However,iftheinterpretationadvancedhereiscorrect,aclearpredictionfollows.Itisinthefemale’sinteresttokeepindividualmalesguessingastotheextenttowhichothermaleshavealsomatedwithher:thelowertheperceptionofthatfrequencythehigheramale’s ,hisestimateofhisownpaternitychances,shouldbe.Hence,femalesshouldbelikelytomatediscreetly,especiallywithsubordinatemales.Wewilldevelopthesamepredictionfromaconsiderationofmatingconflict(seebelow).Toreachthisconclusionwehaveassumedthatthefemalecanbiasthevaluesoftheseestimatesindirectionsfavourabletoher,implyingthatnaturalselectionwasunabletoequipmaleswithbetterassessmentrulesthantheonestheycurrentlyhave.Thus,femalesexualitymayhavebeendesignedtowithholdpotentiallyusefulinformationfrommales.Physiologicalworkdonefromthisperspectivemightberewarding.SEXUALHARASSMENTASANEXPRESSIONOFMATINGCONFLICTMatingconflictbetweenthesexesisnowrecognisedasanintrinsicpartofsexualselection(Hammerstein&Parker,1987).Oneaspectofitconcernsintersexualconflictovertheidentityofeachindividual’smates.Especiallyinsomemammals,thiskindofmatingconflicthasfoundexpressioninsexualcoercionoffemalesbystrongermales.Smuts&Smuts(1993)definedsexualcoercionandconsideredtwocomponents:infanticideandsexualharassment.Manystudentsofprimatebehaviourhavereportedharassmentoraggressiverestrictionofmovementsofsexuallyactive(‘oestrous’)femalesbymales,especiallyhigh-rankingones,sometimestothe 148 00.20.40.6paternityprobability(p)impactoninfantsurvivalFig.8.1.Theimpactofthepaternity( )ofamaleoninfantsurvival.Therelationshiphastheform )(1)(1) gpkAp=ŠŠŠ;theparameterschosenare = 1 = 2 .Asthemale’spaternityincreases,theimpactvariesfromattacktotolerancetoprotection.At ,theinterceptonthey-axisis( ),whichisthemaximumnegativeimpactbyanon-sireonthesurvivaloftheinfant.pointthatthefemaleisinjuredorevenkilled(Smuts&Smuts,1993;chimpanzees:Goodall,1986,p.452;Matsumoto-Oda&Oda,1998;macaques:Chapais,1983;Huffman,1987,1992;Manson,1992,1994;Soltis,1999;hamadryasbaboons:Kummer,1995).Althoughinfanticideisseenbysomeasanextremeformofharassment(Smuts&Smuts,1993),thetwoarenotoftentreatedasbeingdirectlyinterrelatedphenomena.Here,wedevelopamodeltoshowthatatleastsomeofthesexualharassmentinprimatesisdirectlylinkedtofemales’attemptstobepolyandrous.TheoryAssumeamulti-malegroupofaprimatespeciesinwhichfemalesarevulnerabletoinfanticidebymales.Onemaleisdominantandwillguardafemalewhensheisinoestrus,butothermalesarearoundandalsointerestedinmatingwiththefemale.Itisinthefemale’sinteresttodilutethepaternitychancesofthedominantmaleinordertoreducetheriskofinfanticidebymatingpolyandrouslywithothermalesinthegroup,oroccasionallyeveninanadjacentgroup,incaseoneofthemtakesovertopdominanceorouststhecurrentdominant(Hrdy,1979;Hrdy&Whitten,1987;Small,1993;vanSchaiketal.,1999;Soltisetal.,2000;Heistermannetal.,2001).Onemightexpectthatthedominantmalewouldalsobenefitfromreducingtheinfant’sriskofinfanticide.However,wewillnowshowthatthedominantmale’sprobabilityofpaternitythatmaximiseshisfitnessishigherthanthatpreferredbythefemale,andthatheisthereforeexpectedtoattempttopreventmatingsbythefemalewithothermales.Thusamaleisnotonlyincompetitionwithothermales,hemayalsohaveaconflictofinterestwithhismate(s).Amale’sprobabilityofpaternity, ,isthelong-termaverageproportionofinfantssiredbyamaleinsimilarconditions.Weassumethatthevalueof relatedtoamale’sassessmentofit,althoughthisrelationshipmaybeimprecise(seeprevioussection).Ingeneral,amale’sattitudetowardtheinfantisafunctionof ,sothatwithincreasingvaluesof themalechangesfromattack(ifgivenanopportunitytodosoandifprospectsforfuturematingaccesstothefemaleexist),toindifferenceortolerance,andfinallytoanincreasinglystrongtendencytowardprotection(Hrdy,1979).Wecanrepresenttheimpactoninfantsurvivalofthesechangingattitudesas ,withnegativeeffectsatlow p andincreasinglypositiveeffectsas increases(seeFig.8.1).Weexpect toincreasemonotonicallywith ondomain[0,1]andsaturateat ThefunctioninFig.8.1canbeexpressedas: ()(1)(1) gpkAp=ŠŠŠwhere (integer, )isashapeparameterthatdetermineshowfast risesandhowsoonitsaturatesas increases,and and arepositiveconstants(suchthat ).Thevalueof isthemaximumpositiveimpactofthelikelysireontheinfant’ssurvival,whereas canbeseenasthemaximumnegativeimpactofanon-sire(theY- 149 interceptinFig.8.1isat ).Theassumptionof iscriticalfortheresultbelow,butinmakingit,wefollowpreviousanalysesofmaleparentalcare(Harada&Iwasa,1996).Itimpliesthatas increases,thecostsofmaleprotectioneffortsarealsolikelytoriseduetoincreasedcompetitionwithotheractivitiesorincreasedriskofinjury,leadingtoarelativeslowdownininvestmentin,andthuseffectivenessof,protection(inotherwords,onemustassumeprotectioneffortstosaturate).Thequestionisatwhatvalueofthedominantmale’s (herecalled toavoidconfusion)thefitnessofthedominantmaleandofthefemalearemaximised.Ingeneral,femalefitness, ,ismaximisedwhentheinfant’ssurvivalismaximised,assumingtherearenoothermajoreffectsonfitness,suchasvariationinmaleintrinsicgeneticqualityorrelatednesstothefemale.Ontheotherhand,thedominantmale’sfitness, F ,ismaximisedwhen ismaximised,whereasthefitnessofasubordinatemale( F ,ormorepreciselythehighestrankingamongthem)ismaximisedwhen (1)* ismaximised.Wewillnowdevelopexpressionsfor D M F and F Weassumethatinfantsurvivalisafunctionof ,butweightedforthedominantmale’seffectivepower),aswellasafunctionof (1) ,i.e.thestrongestsubordinatemale’spaternity, ,butweightedforhiseffectivepower.Thisyields: ()(1)(1) F gqgq=Š+Š {()(1)(1)} DMF qFqgqgq==Š+Š (1){()(1)(1)} qgqgq=ŠŠ+ŠTheparameterestimatesthemaximumstrengthofthestrongestothermaleinthegrouporthevicinity(i.e.withinthefemale’spotentialpoolofmates).Thisstrengthparameterisbestinterpretedasthelikelihoodthatthismalewillsuccessfullychallengethecurrentdominantinthenearfuture(andthusalsoprotecttheinfantinthefuture),hence .Itspaternityprobabilityofdominantmale(q) 0.20.40.60.8strongestsubordinatemaledominantmaleFig.8.2.Thefitnessofthefemale,dominantmaleandthestrongestsubordinatemaleasafunctionofthepaternity ofthedominantmale(Eq.2-5).Theoptimalfitnessforeachofthethreeisatdifferentvaluesof ,whichcannotbesatisfiedsimultaneously,reflectingaconflict.complement,1,representstheprobabilitythatthedominantmalewillbeabletowithstandchallengestohisdominantposition,andhencealsohisabilitytoprotecttheinfantagainstinfanticidalattacksbyanyofthesemales(oryetothers).Figure8.2illustratestheresultingmatingconflict:thedominantmale’sfitnessismaximisedatahighervalueof comparedtothevalueof atwhichthefemale’sfitnessismaximised.WedidnotfindanalyticalsolutionstoEqs.3-5satisfyingallvaluesof .However,for ,thevalueof atwhichthefemale’sfitnessismaximised( )isat1;inotherwords,sheisexpectedtofavourmatingsbyothermalesinproportiontotheirrelativestrength.Stillassuming =, D M F ,thedominantmale’sfitness,ismaximizedat 2133(1)4 33(1)(1)  ==Š++Š orat ,whicheveristhesmaller.Itcaneasilybeshownthat q isgreaterthan(inotherwords,thereisaconflictofinterestbetweenthefemaleandthedominantmale),provided 150 thefactor ()(1) .Thefactor (1) reachesitsmaximumvalueat whichmeansthatif ()0.25 theconflictisguaranteedforallvaluesof ;for ()0.25 thematingconflictmightstillexistbutisnotguaranteed,sinceitwilldependonthevalueof .Forvaluesof ,wefoundmatingconflictinallnumericalsolutionsthatweattemptedforrealisticvaluesof and ,and suchthat ()0.25 Ofcourse,thisresultraisesthequestionastotheinterpretationof ()0.25 .Thisinequalitywilloftenholdbecauseprotectionisagainstinfanticidalattacks;hence,ifprotectionweretotallyeffective, .Sinceinfanticidemostoftenhappenswhenthedominantmaleiseliminatedorincapacitated,thisimpliesthatnormally iscloseto ,i.e. 0.25 .Hence,forallrealisticrangesofvaluesofallthreeparameters,therewillbeamatingconflictbetweenthedominantmaleinthegrouporneighbourhoodandthefertilefemale.AsimpliedbyFigure8.2,amatingconflictexistsbetweenthefemaleandthesubordinate/peripheralmalesaswell,becausethedominantmale’spaternityathermaximumfitness, ,ishigherthanthatofthebestsubordinatemale(thegraphshowshisoptimumintermsofthedominantmale’spaternity;hisownoptimumisthecomplementofthatvalue).However,thebehaviouralexpressionofthisconflictisusuallypreemptedbymatingcompetitionbetweenthedominantandthesubordinatemales,forcingthelattertomatemuchlessthantheywouldotherwisedo.Hence,thesubordinatesneverreachthezoneinwhichfemaleswouldprefertomatelesswiththem.Indeed,thesubordinatemalesreachtheiroptimumpaternity, ,atavaluemuchlessthan1,becausethedominantmaleisexpectedtoattackinfantsobviouslysiredbythem.Ontheotherhand,whendominantmalesarenotaround,forinstancebecausefemalesarerathersolitary,theytooareexpectedtoharassoestrousfemales.Inourequationsweonlyincorporatedtheroleof Fortheunrealisticcaseof (seeabove),thereisnomatingconflict,withbothsexesreachingtheirhighestfitnessat onesubordinatemale,theonemostlikelytosucceedinchallengingthedominantmaleinthenearfuture.Ifonlyoneclearlystronger,youngsubordinatemaleresidesinthegroup,thisapproachisacceptable,becauseoneexpectsfemalestorecognisesuchmales.Forinstance,malebaboonsabouttoriseindominancerankhavedifferentbehaviouralstylesandendocrineprofilesfromothersofsimilarrankwellbeforetheiractualrise(Virgin&Sapolsky,1997).However,iftherearemoresuchmales(andfemalesrecognisethemassuch),ourtreatmentisconservativeandmatingconflictbetweenthefemaleandthedominantmaleisevenmoreintense.Forinstance,iftwomaleshaveareasonablechanceofupsettingthedominantmale(i.e.with ),thefemale’sfitnesswillmaximiseat 1 PredictionsandevidenceAbasic,albeittrivial,predictionisthat,ifnoothermalesareinthematingpool,i.e. ,boththedominantmaleandthefemalewillreachtheiroptimumfitnessat .Hence,intheabsenceofothermales,nomatingconflictisexpected,andthusnoharassmentbythedominantmale.Whenothermalesarepresent,however,variouspredictionscanbemade.Wedevelopthemhereandalsoofferapreliminaryevaluationoftheirfitwiththeprimateliterature.1.HarassmentbydominantmalesObservationsofharassmentofoestrousfemales,especiallybyhigh-rankingmales,inspiredthedevelopmentofthemodel.Itsfundamentalpredictionisthattheconflictofinterestbetweenthefemaleandthedominantmalemayfindexpressioninabehaviouralconflictifthemalehasthemeanstocoercethefemale,e.g.bycoercivemateguarding,andthefemalewilltrytoescapeinordertomatewithothermales(which,accordingtothismodel,she 151 islikelytodo).Thisisastrongpredictionbecauseitcontrastswiththenaiveexpectationthatthemalesmostlikelytoforcematingswillbenon-preferred,andhencegenerallysubordinate.Onemightobjectthatattackingthefemaleinsteadoftherivalmaleofamatingorconsortingpairissimplytheleastriskyoptionforadominantmale,andisalsolikelytopreventthefemalefrommatingwithathirdmalewhileheisengagedinfighting.However,thereisabundantevidenceforharassmentoffertilefemalesbymaleswhenthefemaleisnotactuallymatingorevennearanothermale(seeSmuts&Smuts,1993;andseebelow).Moreover,ifdominantmalesarenotnearthefertilefemale,lower-rankingmalesarealsoexpectedtoemployharassment.Thus,inbothchimpanzees(troglodytes)andorangutans(Pongopygmaeuspossessivemateguardingandforcedmatings,respectively,bysuchnon-dominantmalesarecommonlyobserved(Tutin,1979;Mitani,1985;Goodall,1986,pp.457-64;Schürmann&vanHooff,1986;Fox,1998).2.FemalepolyandryinrelationtothenumberofmalesThematingconflictmodelindicatesthatastheeffectivepowerofthedominantmale1declines,theconflictofinterestbetweenthedominantmaleandthefemalewillincrease(seeFig.8.3).Onecommonsourceofthereducedpowerofthedominantisanincreasednumberofmalesinthematingpool,becausethisprobablyincreasesthestrengthofthestrongestamongthemandperhapsalsobecausethelargernumberitselfmaywearoutthedominantmale,eitherdirectlyorbecausecoalitionsaremorelikely(Bercovitch,1989;Noë&Sluyter,1990).Themodelpredictsthatifthefemalewinstheconflict,theconcentrationofpaternityinthetop- Notethatharassmentofmatingpairsbyjuveniles(forexampleDrukkeretal.,1991)andfemales(Linnetal.,1995)alsooccurs.Suchharassmentisofcoursenotexplainedbythematingconflictmodel.matingconflict 0.00.20.40.60.8Fig.8.3.Themagnitudeofthematingconflict( D ,seethetext)betweenthedominantmaleandthefemaleasafunctionofthestrengthofthestrongestsubordinatemale().Theconflictbetweenthedominantmaleandthefemaleclearlyincreaseswith,albeitnotmonotonically.rankingmalewilldeclineasthenumberorstrengthofrivalsincreases.Thefemalemayachievethisoutcomebymatingmorepolyandrouslyduringfertilecyclesorbymatingafterconception.ThematingconflictmodelthusoffersanamendmenttotheexplanationforthedistributionofpaternitiesovertheavailablemalesprovidedbythePriority-of-Access(PoA)model(Altmann,1962).AccordingtothePoAmodel,adominantmaleexcludesothermalesfrommatingaslongasthereisonlyonefemalenearovulation.Lackofabsoluteconcentrationofpaternityindominantmaleswouldbeduetoareductioninmalematingmonopolyattimesofoverlapoffemaleoestrousperiodsorhighintruderpressure.Themating-conflictmodelclaimsthatthispatternisduetoactivefemalepolyandry,whereasPoAassumesfemalesdonotactivelyseekpolyandry.PoAalsoassumesthatmale-maleaggressionservesonlytomonopoliseaccesstofemales,andisthereforeconsistentwithattacksonmatingpairs,butnotwithmaleaggressiontargetedatthefemaleinparticular(seealsovanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,thisvolume).Todistinguishbetweenthetwomodels,detaileddataon(changesin)male 152 dominancerelationsaswellasmatingsandtheirtiming(i.e.thenumberoffemalessexuallyactivesimultaneously)areneeded.Ifchallengermalesfromwithinthegrouparemorelikelytosucceedthanrecentimmigrants,asisoftenfound(Henzi&Lucas,1980;Cheney,1983;vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,1985,2001;Robinson,1988;Perry,1998),asubsidiarypredictionfollows:femalesmayprefertoliveinmulti-malegroups,allotherthingsbeingequalbecausethe‘insider’malesposelessofaninfanticideriskprovidedthattheyweregrantedashareofthematings.Thisideahasbeensuggestedbeforebutremainshardtotest(cf.vanSchaik,1996;Nunn&vanSchaik,2000).However,ouranalysisheresuggeststhateventhedominantmalemayfinditinhisinteresttotoleratesomeunrelatedsubordinatemales,becausehisoptimumpaternitymaybeslightlylessthan1,assuminghecancomecloseenoughtothisvalueinreality.3.FemalepolyandryinrelationtopotentialchangeofmaledominancerelationsTheeffectivepowerofthedominantmalemaybereducedbyincreasedstrengthofoneormoreofthesubordinatemales,increasingtheriskofaneffectivechallenge.Ifafemalecanrecognisethatthecurrenttop-rankingmaleislikelytobedefeatedbeforethebirthofherinfant,sheshouldselectivelydecreasehermatingswithhim.Thisscenarioassumesthatthetoprankamongmalesisacquiredthroughchallengeandnotbysuccession,asinsomemacaquespecieswithlargegroupsandseasonalbreeding(reviewedinvanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,thisvolume).Inmanyprimatespeciesfemalesareknowntoattempttobreakawayfromthemonopolisationofthedominantmalesandactivelyattempttomatewithsubordinateorperipheralmales(Hrdy,1981;Small,1993).Theprimateliteraturecontainsafewreportsthatfemalesaremoreactivelypolyandrouswhenthedominancesituationamongthehigh-rankingmalesisnotstable(Samuelsetal.,1984;vanNoordwijk,1985;Janson,fideManson,1995;Mansonetal.1997;Albertsetal.,2003)orwhenthesinglemaleinthegroupisweak(Agoramoorthy&Hsu,2000).Amorerefinedpredictionisthatweexpectfemalestoattempttomatepreferentiallywiththosesubordinateorperipheralmalesmostlikelytosuccessfullychallengethecurrentdominantmaleinthefuture.Hence,oestrousfemalesshouldnotseekmatingswithothermalesthanthedominantrandomlybutshowdistinctpreferences,whichshouldbelinkedtothetargetmales’prospectsforfuturedominance.Inseveralpopulationsyoungmaturingmalesareknowntorapidlyriseinrankandtakeovertoprank,surpassingseveralmalesoveraperiodofonlyafewmonths(e.g.,Cheney,1983;vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,1985,1988,2001;Hamilton&Bulger,1990;Virgin&Sapolsky,1997;Soltisetal.,2000).Sincesuchchallengesbymaturingmalesareratherpredictable,weexpectthefuturetop-rankingmale(ifalreadypresentinthegroup)tohavealargershareofthematingsthanexpectedforhiscurrentdominanceposition.Atleastonestudytodateseemstoconfirmthispoint:Smith(1994)reportsforalargecaptivegroupofrhesusmacaquesthathighmalesiringsuccess,attributedtofemalechoice,precededarisetohighdominancerankforyoungmales.4.SurreptitiousmatingwithsubordinatemalesThenon-zerovalueof thatmaximizesthesubordinatemales’fitnessleadstothepredictionthatitisintheinterestofboththefemaleandthesubordinatemaletomateasinconspicuouslyaspossibleinordertopreventthedominantmalefromadjustinghis estimatedown,andwithholdprotectionfromtheinfant.Wethereforeexpectthatmatingsbetweenfemalesandsubordinatemalestendtotakeplaceoutofsightofthedominantmale,e.g.attheperipheryorawayfromthegroup,andshouldlessoftenbeaccompaniedbycalls.Inordertodistinguishthispatternfromgeneralmatingcompetition,itshouldevenhappenifthedominantmaleisinvisiblecontactbuttoofarawaytoattackthepaireffectively.Wecanfurtherpredictthatinconspicuousmating 153 withsubordinatesisevenfoundinspecieswithouteffectivemaleharassmentoffemales.Despiteaseriouslackofquantitativedata,ithasbeennotedforseveralspeciesthatmatingsbetweenfemalesandsubordinatemalestendtooccurrathersurreptitiously(Pantroglodytes:Tutin,1979;Goodall,1986;Macacafuscata:Huffman,1992;mulatta:Berardetal.,1994;M.arctoidesNieuwenhuijsenetal.,1986;M.sylvanus:Paul,1989).Onestudyfocusingonmatingsinconcealedplacesnotedthatespeciallylower-rankingmalesfasciculariswereinvolvedinmatingsoutsideofvisualcontactwiththerestofthegroup(Gygax,1995),aspredicted.Inmanyspeciesfemalesgiveaspecificcopulationcall,towhichothermalesdorespondbylookingatthepair(vanNoordwijk,1985)orevenattackingthem(Oda&Masataka,1995).Inatleastonestudyatendencywasfoundformatingswithsubordinatemalestobequiet(M.thibetana:Zhao,,althoughanotherstudyfoundnoeffectofmaleidentityonfemalecallingtendency(fascicularis:vanNoordwijk,1985).Hence,renewedexaminationofthepatternsindiscreetmatingsmaybeworthwhile,alsoinspecieswithouteffectivemaleharassment(aslongasinfanticidebymalesposesarisk).SEXUALHARASSMENTANDREPRODUC-TIVEPHYSIOLOGYThedistributionofsexualharassmentThemodelpresentedaboveconfirmstheexistenceofaconflictofinterestbetweenthebreedingfemaleandthegroup’smales.Theconflictwiththedominantmale(s)ismostlikelytobeexpressedbecausethismale(ormales)willtrytomonopolisethefemalemostofthetime,thuspreemptinganyconflictwiththeothermales.Thequestion,ofcourse,iswhowinsthisconflict?Whenevermaleharassmentoccurs,femalescanonlywinatseriouscosts.Coercivemateguardingmakesitmoredifficult,andmorecostly,forthefemaletoseekthematingswithsubordinateandperipheralmalesneededtoachievethedistributionof valuesoptimaltoher.Harassmentismorecommonwherefemalesarelesspowerful,bothphysicallyandsocially(Smuts&Smuts,1993).Givingintocoercionmakesthefemalemorevulnerabletoinfanticide,andifthisriskissufficientlyincreased,weexpectthatnaturalselectionhasproducedphysiologicalorbehaviouraltendenciesinfemalestoreducethedominantmale’smonopolisationpotential,providedthattheircostsdonotexceedthegainsofreducedinfanticiderates.Sinceitisnotaprioriclearwhatformsthesefemalecounter-strategiescantake,wewillfirstexaminethetaxonomicdistributionofsexualharassmentofsexuallyactive(‘oestrous’)femalesinprimates,buildingonexistingreviews(especiallySmuts&Smuts,1993;Dixson,1998),andthensearchforderivedreproductivefeaturesinthetaxawithharassment.Inordertocapturethevariationinmalebehavioursdirectedatsexuallyactiveor‘oestrous’females,weproposethefollowingcategoriesofsexualharassment(definedasaggressionbysexuallymaturemalesagainstsexuallyactiveor‘oestrous’females):(1)Nosexualharassment,noranyattempts,reported.(2)Sexualharassmentattemptsreported,butonlyinthedirectmatingcontextandmostlyineffective,i.e.thefemalewardsoffthemaleorcounterattacks,andthemaleisunabletopreventthefemalefrommovingawayormatingwithothers.(3)Effectivesexualharassmentisobservedinboththedirectmatingcontextandofoestrousfemalesingeneral,asevidencedbycoercivemateguardingandphysicalattacksontheoestrousfemalefollowedbysubmission.Malebehaviourssometimesincludebitesthatresultinwoundingorevendeath,forcedmatings,andattackson 154 femaleswhenmatingwithothermales.Thesecondcategoryisneededbecauseinseveralspecies(includingalsomanynon-primatemammals),malesshowaggressiontowardfemalesinthematingcontextthatisnotaccompaniedbyanyattemptsatcoercivemateguarding.Thepresenceofthesebehaviourssuggeststhataggressionmaybeanintegralcomponentofmatinginthesespecies.Theextenttowhichtheseattemptsatharassmentwouldconstitutedefactoharassmentdependsonthedegreetowhichtheystopfemalesfromachievingthepreferreddegreeofpolyandry.Theliteratureisunderstandablyvagueonthis,butitisourimpressionthatfemalescanstillmatewithothermales.Inanycase,itisusefultokeepthissecondcategoryseparatefromthecaseswheremalesalsoattackoestrousfemalesoutsidethedirectmatingcontextorforcematings,thecategoryofeffectiveharassment.Forthepresentpurpose,welimitourreviewtospeciesvulnerabletoinfanticide.Aspeciesisconsideredvulnerableifinfanticidebymalesisreportedforit,orifithasalifehistorythatmakesthefemalesofthespeciesvulnerabletosuchattacks,orboth(vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,2000;vanSchaik,2000b).Theadvantageofthisdefinitionisthatweneednotrelyonlyonreportsofinfanticide,whichtendtoberare.Asrequired,allspeciesknowntohaveinfanticidearealsopredictedbythelifehistorymeasuretobevulnerable.Althoughdataonharassmentarestillveryincompleteafewclearpatternsemerge.Consistentwithearliercompilations,weseenoevidenceforeffectiveharassmentinLemuroidea(lemurs)orinPlatyrrhini(NewWorldprimates),butmanyreportsfortheCatarrhini(OldWorldprimates;Fig.8.4).Examplesofadultfemalesbeingabletosystematicallyelicitsubmissionfromadultmalesaremostcommonlyfoundinlemurs,inseveralmonomorphicNewWorldprimatesandinthefewpair-livingOldWorldprimates(Kappeler,1993;Strier,1994).Inallthesespecies,malesattackrivalmalesbutweseeatmostattemptsatharassmentof%speciesw/harassment LEMPLACAT Fig.8.4.Theincidenceofeffectivesexualharassment(whichmayincludeinjuryoffemalesandforcedmatings)amongprimatespeciesvulnerabletoinfanticidebymalesinthreeradiations(LEM=Lemuroidea;PLA=Platyrrhini-NewWorldprimates;CAT=Catarrhini-OldWorldprimates).Basedonliteraturereview(startingwithSmuts&Smuts,1993),availablefromtheauthorsuponrequest.Numberofspecieswithinformationindicatedabovecolumns.females,e.g.inlemurs(e.g.,Pereira&Weiss,1991;Sauther,1991;Brockman,1999),andfemalesoftencounter-attackandbeabletochoosetheirmates(Richard,1992).Likewise,onlyinsomeNewWorldprimatesdoweseeevidenceofcoalitionsofmalesbeingrequiredtoinspectafemale’ssexualstate(inSaimirioerstediAtelesLagothrix:Boinski,1987;Smuts&Smuts,1993).Remarkably,effectivesexualharassmentisabsentamongNewWorldprimates,eventhemoredimorphicones:forinstance,femaleAlouattapalliatasuccessfullyrebuffattemptsatforcedmatingsbymales(Jones,1985).Conversely,effectivesexualharassmentbymalesisreportedformanyOldWorldprimatespecies.Inthislineage,wealsoencountermanyrecordsofmalesgenerallyharassingfemales,orfemalesrequiringcoalitionsinordertodefendthemselvesagainstharassingmales(Smuts,1987;Smuts&Smuts,1993).However,bynomeansallOldWorldprimatespeciesshowsexualharassmentbymales.Oursampleisasyettooincompletetoallowanalysisoftheinterspecificpatterninsexualharassmentin 155 %ofspeciesphase(days) (b)5 PLACATdays(median)%ofspecies 510152025 05101520253822 LEMPLACATFig.8.5.Variationamongspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticidebymalesinthreeprimateradiations(seeFig.4forabbreviations)inavarietyofsexualfeatures:(a)themediandurationofthemating(‘oestrous’)period(P0.001);(b)themeandurationofthefollicularphase(indays)oftheovariancycle(P0.01);(c)thepercentageofspeciesshowingexaggeratedsexualswellings(P0.001);(d)thepercentageofspecieswithmatingcallsbyfemales(P0.01).TestedwithKruskal-Wallisone-wayAnova.Numbersofspeciesindicatedabovecolumns.(a,c,anddbasedondatacompiledinvanSchaiketal.,1999;bondatacompiledbyvanSchaiketal.,2000.)relationtotheknownriskfactors(largermalebodysizeorweaponry,lackoffemaleallies:Smuts&Smuts,1993).Lemursarelargelymonomorphic(Kappeler,1991)andNewWorldprimatesarefarlessdimorphicoverallthanOldWorldprimates(e.g.,vanSchaiketal.,2000).However,additionalfactorsareprobablyinvolvedbecauseclearlydimorphicNewWorldprimatespeciesandevensomehighlydimorphicOldWorldspecies(e.g.Erythrocebuspatas)failtoproduceclearevidenceofsexualharassment.Hence,asyetunidentifiedadditionalfactorsarealsoinvolvedinshapinginterspecificvariationintheoccurrenceandintensityofsexualharassment.Femalecounter-strategiestomalesexualharassmentHavingfoundthateffectivesexualharassmentoffemalesbymalesislimitedtoOldWorldprimates,weemployatwo-stepproceduretoidentifyderivedfeaturesofsexualityinOldWorldprimatesthatpossiblyrepresentcounter-strategiestothegreaterriskofsexualharassment.ThefirststepisasystematiccomparisonoffemalesexualityinspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticideinthethreeprimateradiationscomparedaboveandseeinwhichrespectOldWorldprimatesstandout.ThesecondstepistoconductcomparisonswithintheOldWorldprimateswithvariationincoerciveabilitiesorinfanticiderisk 156 matingperiod(estrus’)(days) nofmatespercycleCATLEM+ Fig.8.6.Mean(±s.d.)durationofthemating(‘oestrous’)periodinspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticidebymalesinrelationtothedegreeoffemalepolyandry,inthosevulnerabletomalesexualharassment(CAT)andthosenotvulnerable(LEMandPLA).Degreeofpolyandry:1-matingwithasinglemalein�90%ofcycles;m-matingwithmultiplemalesin10-50%ofcycles;andM-matingwithmultiplemalesinmorethan50%ofthecycles(cf.vanNoordwijk&vanSchaik,2000).Samplesizes(numberofspecies)indicatedateachpoint.DataonmatingperiodtakenfromvanSchaiketal.(1999).Rankcorrelationissignificant(P0.01)forcatarrhines.Thematingperiod(‘oestrous’period,i.e.periodbetweenfirstandlastmating)withinanovariancycleisatleastthreetimeslongerintheaverageOldWorldprimatethanintheotherradiations(Fig.8.5a).Moreover,matingperiodincreaseswiththenumberofmalesthatafemalesendsupmatingwith,inOldWorldprimatesbutnotinothers(Fig.8.6),consistentwiththeexpectationthatonlyinsomeOldWorldprimatesfemaleswouldfinditdifficulttoescapefromthemonopolisationofthedominantmaleinordertomatewithothermales.ThelongmatingperiodsinOldWorldprimatesaremadepossiblebyachangeintheovariancycle,inthattheirfollicularphasesareabouttwiceaslongasintheotherradiations(Fig.8.5b).ComparisonswithintheOldWorldprimatessuggestedthatfollicularphasesarelongerwherethesexesaremoredimorphicinbodysizeorweaponry;specifically,wefoundevidenceforcorrelatedevolutionbetweenthelengthofthefollicularphaseandthedegreeofcaninedimorphism(vanSchaiketal.,2000),suggestingthatthefactorexplainingvariationamongradiationsisalsoatworkwithintheOldWorldprimateslineage.ThelongperiodsofsexualactivityshownbyOldWorldprimatefemalesrequireeitherthatspermremainsviableinthefemalereproductivetractforlongperiodsoftime,orthatovulationislesstightlylinkedtothevisual,olfactoryorbehaviouralsignalsthanusuallyassumed,orboth(cf.Martin,1992).Thereisnoworkinsupportoftheformerpossibility,butrecentworkhassupportedthesecondidea.Nunn(1999)andvanSchaiketal.(2000)reviewendocrinologicalworkshowingthatovulationisonlypoorlylinkedtothesesignalsandthusshouldberatherunpredictableformales.RecentworkbyHeistermannetal.(2001)onhanumanlangursconfirmedthisprediction,showingthatovulationcantakeplacewithapproximatelyequalprobabilityatanytimeinthevariableperiodoffemalesexualactivityandthatmalesdonotrecognisethefemale’stimeofovulation,bothintermsofbehaviourandofthedegreetowhichpaternityisconcentratedinthedominant.Ifunpredictabletimingofovulationwasfavoured 157 S.D.cyclelength 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 CAT LEM PLA Fig.8.7.Thestandarddeviationofthelengthoftheovariancycleinspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticideinthreeprimateradiations(seeFig.8.4).Numberofspeciesindicatedabovecolumns.Arrowsindicatemedians.TestedwithKruskal-Wallisone-wayAnova(P0.01).bynaturalselectionbecauseitallowedthefemaletohavelongermatingperiodsofvariablelength,thenthereshouldbelessneedforitamongthespeciesthat,whilevulnerabletoinfanticide,arenotvulnerabletomalesexualharassment,i.e.lemursandNewWorldprimates.Thereareasyetnodetailedstudiesofthekinddoneonlangursforanyofthesespecies.However,wecangetanindicationoftheextenttowhichovulationisunpredictablebyexaminingthevariabilityinthedurationofthefollicularphaseoftheovariancycle(inmostspecies,malescanrecognizewhenthelutealphaseiswellunderway;e.g.,Dixson,1998).Dataonvariationinfollicularphaselengtharescarce,butbecausethevariabilityinthefollicularphaseexceedsthatinthelutealphase(onaveragebyaboutafactoroftwo:seecompilationinvanSchaiketal.,2000),variabilityinthetotaldurationoftheovariancyclemayprovidearoughindicationofunpredictableovulation.Figure8.7showsthatthestandarddeviationofthelengthoftheovariancycleinoursample(basedonHayssenal.,1993andacompilationbyK.Hodges&U.Moehle,unpubl.)isindeedhigherforOldWorldprimatesthanitisforthetwootherradiations(includingonlyspeciesvulnerabletoinfanticide).Thedifferencesamongthemaresignificant(Kruskal-Wallistest,H[2]=11.37;P0.01),andintheexpecteddirection.OldWorldprimatesdifferfromtheotherradiationsintwomoreaspectsofsexualbehaviour:sexualswellingsandcopulationcalls.ThefirsthasgeneratedmuchinterestfromDarwin(1876)on(e.g.,Dixson,1983;Hrdy,1997;Nunn,1999):femalesofseveralspecieshaveexaggeratedsexualswellings(Fig.8.5c).AmongOldWorldprimatestheseswellingsarefoundonlyinspeciesthatareactuallyorpotentiallypolyandrous(cf.Clutton-Brock&Harvey,1976),andamongthempredominantlyinnon-seasonalbreeders(vanSchaiketal.,1999).TherestrictiontoOldWorldprimatesandthepredominanceamongnon-seasonalbreedersisconsistentwiththehypothesisthattheseswellingsareneededwheretheriskofharassmentbydominantmalesisparticularlyserious(femalesinseasonalbreederscanmovemorefreelyfrommaletomalebecausetheytendtobesexuallyactivesimultaneously).Thus,asarguedbyNunn’s(1999)graded-signalhypothesis,sexualswellingsfunctionnotonlytoattractthedominantmale(s)duringtheirmaximumsize(whentheprobabilityofovulationishighest),butthroughtheirexaggerationtheycanalsoattractlower-rankingorperipheralmaleswhenthedominantmalesarelessattractedduetothecoststothesemalesofmateguardingfortoolong.ForfurtherdiscussionofexaggeratedswellingsseeZinneretal.(thisvolume).Second,wefindthatfemalesmatingcallsaremorecommonamongOldWorldprimates(Fig.8.5d).Matingcallsaregivenduringorafterejaculation.Theyarenottobeconfusedwith‘oestrouscalls’givenwhenthefemaleisreceptivebeforeactuallymating(generallyinterpretedasalertingallmaleswithinhearingdistancetohercondition),orwith‘distress’callsgivenbyfemalenorthernelephantsealsorchickenswhenmountedbyasubordinatemaleoratatimethefemaleisnotreceptive,oftenfollowedbyinterferencebyahigher-rankingmale(Cox&LeBoeuf,1977;Pizzari& 158 Birkhead,2000).Matingcallsmayalertothermalesthatthefemalehasmated,andmaythusattractthem(O’Connell&Cowlishaw,1994;vanSchaiketal.1999).Matingcallsmaybeagradedsignalwhosequalityvariesthroughoutthecycle(Semple&McComb,2000)andthussomehowindicatestheprobabilityofovulation,muchlikeswellings(theirdistributionacrossspecieslargelymirrorsthatofswellings:vanSchaiketal.,1999).Femalesmayalsousethemtactically,callingonlyormoreoftenwithparticularmalesthanwithothers(seeabove).Moredetailedfieldstudiesandcomparisonsbetweenspecies,includingseveralNewWorldprimates,mayhelpusdeterminewhethertheirfunctionis,asproposedhere,linkedtothereductionofmalesexualharassment.Thuswefoundstrongevidenceforalinkbetweenthedistributionofphysiological,morphologicalandbehaviouralcharactersticsoffemalesexualitywiththeoccurrenceofharassmentbymaleswhoarephysicallystronger.LineagedifferencesinsexualityThesignificantdifferencesbetweenradiationsinvariousfeaturesservetoshowtheexistenceofagradeshift.Explaininggradeshiftsisinherentlydifficultbecausemanyfactorshavechangedinparallel.Forinstance,amongOldWorldprimates,wesee,relativetothetwootherradiations,thepresenceofterrestrialadaptations,largermeanbodysize(relativetoextantspeciesintheotherlineages),largermeangroupsize,ageneralreductioninrelianceonolfactorycommunication,greatertendenciestowardfolivory(relativetoNewWorldprimates),etc.ItisofcourseentirelypossiblethatoneoracombinationofthesefactorsfacilitatedtheevolutionoflongerfollicularphasesorexaggeratedswellingsinOldWorldprimates.However,thevariationwithinthelineage,withrespecttothedurationoffollicularphasesinrelationtomaleharassmentpotential,tothedurationofmatingperiodinrelationtopotentialforpolyandry,andtotheconditionsinwhichexaggeratedswellingsoccur,suggeststhatsubsequentevolutionwithinthelineageinthesetraitsdidcorrelatewithvariationinharassmentpotential.Thus,variationwithinthelineageisinthesamedirectionasthatbetweenthelineages.Wethereforebelievethattheideathatthecombinationofmaleharassmentandinfanticideriskfacilitatedtheevolutionofthesederivedsexualfeaturesisaviableworkinghypothesis,worthyoffurtherevaluation.DISCUSSIONANDPROSPECTSItshouldbestressedthatwedonotproposethatprimatesexualityismoldedbyinfanticideriskalone:therearenumerousselectivefactorsonwhatisperhapsthebehaviourmosttightlylinkedtofitnessandthusmostdirectlyresponsivetonaturalselection(Dixson,1998).Birkhead&Kappeler(thisvolume)discussadditionalhypothesesforfemalepolyandryinprimates,butthesearenotnecessarilyincompatiblewiththeinfanticideavoidancefunction.Nonetheless,theaimofthischapterhasbeentoevaluateHrdy’s(1979)predictionsconcerningtheimpactofinfanticideriskonsexualbehaviourandreproductivephysiologyinprimates.Theoriginalpredictionsareupheldbutalinktosexualharassmentcanalsobeaddedtothelist.Ourpreliminaryexplorationsuggeststhatharassmenthasimpactedfemalereproductivebehaviourandphysiologybutasalways,theroleofcomparativeworkisbestregardedascomplementarytofocusedobservationalorexperimentalstudies.Thestudiesusedtoevaluatethehypotheseswerenotdesignedwiththeseideasinmind,andconfoundingfactorscanthereforerarelybeexcluded.Wethereforemakethefollowingsuggestionsforfuturework.Moredetailedstudiesoffemalesexualbehaviorcouldincorporatetheroleofsocialdynamicsintheoptimalbalancebetweenpaternityconcentrationindominantsanddilutionormanipulationofits 159 assessment.Forexample,ifnewdominantsarealwaysrecentimmigrants,verylittlepaternitydilutionisexpected,butifnewdominantsarealwayslong-termresidens,muchmorepaternitydilutionisexpected.StrongtestswouldcorrectfortheexpectationbasedonthePriority-of-Accessmodel.AstomaledecisionmakingitwillbedifficulttoimproveonthebehaviouralmonitoringofBorriesal.(1999)onhanumanlangurs,althoughadditionalworkondifferentspecieswouldbeworthwhile.Withrespecttomatingconflict,detailedstudiessuchasthoseofGygax(1995)onthetendencyofparticularindividualsorpairstoengageinmatingsinconcealedlocationsoronthefemales’tendencytogivecopulationcallscanbeconducted.Futureworkonfemalereproductivephysiologycouldfocusonthreeissues.First,detailedworkonthesexualbehaviouroffemalesinspeciesinthedifferentradiationsalongthelinesofthestudybyHeistermannetal.(2001)isneeded,wheresocialandsexualbehaviour,femaleovarianstateandpaternityareallrecordedsimultaneously.Second,weshouldstudy(ifnecessarybyexperimentalmanipulation)theimpactofmalerepresentationinthesocialunitonfemalereproductivephysiologyandbehaviourinspeciesofdifferentlineages.Third,morefine-grainedcomparativeworkontherelationshipbetweensexualharassmentandpossiblecoercionindicatorsinOldWorldprimates,aswellasdetailedstudiesofhighlydimorphicNewWorldprimates,mayhelpusunderstandthedistributionofmalesexualharassmentinrelationtofemalereproductivephysiology.SUMMARY&CONCLUSIONSWeexploredthehypothesisthatthevulnerabilityoffemalestoinfanticidebymaleshasaffectedfemalesexualbehaviourandgivenrisetosexualharassmentinmanyprimates.Afterestablishingtheadaptivenatureofinfanticidalbehaviourformaleswhoattackinfantstheydidnotfather,webrieflyreviewedsexualcounter-strategiesbyfemales(polyandrousmating,matingduringpregnancy).Wethenaddressedtwomainissues.First,becausepaternitydistributionisaconstant-sumgame,afemalefacesaconsiderablechallenge.Wearguedthatthefemale’ssexualbehaviourservestoproducepaternityestimatesamongthevariousmalesthatadduptomorethan1.Moreover,paternityuncertaintymayforcemalesintoavoidingthecostliestmistakes,makingthemrefrainfromattackinginfantstheyprobablydidnotsireandmakingthemprotectthosetheymayhavesired.Second,weproposedanexplanationthatlinksinfanticidewiththecommonobservationthathigh-rankingmalesoftenharassoestrousfemales.Wedevelopedamodelthatshowsthatamatingconflictexistsbetweenthefemaleandthedominantmale(s),andexaminedseveraldetailedpredictionsaboutmatingbehaviour.Matingconflictcanfindexpressioninharassmentifmalescancoercefemales.Thismodelledtoseveralpredictions,andapreliminaryevaluationshowedagoodfitwithexistingdataonprimatesexualbehaviour.WealsonotedthateffectivesexualharassmentamongprimatesisprobablylimitedtoOldWorldprimates,inwhichitmayhaveproducedanarmsrace,leadingtofurtherchangesinfemalesexualbehaviourandphysiologyinthislineage,includinglongermatingperiods,longerfollicularphaseswithmoreunpredictableovulation,exaggeratedsexualswellingsandperhapsmoreextensivefemalecopulationvocalisations.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSWethankKeithHodgesandUlrikeMoehleforallowingustousetheircompilationofdataonprimateovariancycles,HelenZayacforcompilingmanyoftheharassmentreferences,andTimBirkhead,PattyGowaty,SarahHrdy,PeterKappeler,RebeccaLewis,andSagarPanditforhelpfulcomments. 160 Agoramoorthy,G.&Hsu,M.J.2000.ExtragroupcopulationamongwildredhowlermonkeysinVenezuela.FoliaPrimatologica,147-51.Alberts,S.C.,Watts,H.&Altmann,J.2003.Queuingandqueuejumping:longtermpatternsofdominancerankandmatingsuccessinmalesavannahbaboons.AnimalBehaviour,inpress.Altmann,S.A.1962.Afieldstudyofthesociobiologyoftherhesusmonkey,MacacamulattaAnnalsoftheNewYorkAcademyofSciences,338-435.Angst,W.&Thommen,D.1977.Newdataandadiscussionofinfantkillinginoldworldmonkeysandapes.FoliaPrimatologica,198-229.Bartlett,T.Q.,Sussman,R.W.&Cheverud,J.M.1993.Infantkillinginprimates:areviewofobservedcaseswithspecificreferencetothesexualselectionhypothesis.AmericanAnthropologist,958-90.Berard,J.D.,Nürnberg,P.,Epplen,J.T.&Schmidtke,J.1994.Alternativereproductivetacticsandreproductivesuccessinmalerhesusmacaques.Behaviour,177-201.Bercovitch,F.B.1989.Bodysize,spermcompetition,anddeterminantsofreproductivesuccessinmalesavannababoons.Evolution,1507-21.Boggess,J.1984.InfantkillingandmalereproductivestrategiesinlangursPresbytisentellus.In:Infanticide:ComparativeandEvolutionaryPerspectives,ed.G.Hausfater&S.B.Hrdy,pp.283-310.NewYork:AldinedeGruyter.Boinski,S.1987.BirthsynchronyinsquirrelmonkeysSaimirioerstediBehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,393-400.Borries,C.1997.InfanticideinseasonallybreedingmultimalegroupsofHanumanlangursPresbytisentellusinRamnagarSouthNepal.BehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,139-50.-2000.MaledispersalandmatingseasoninfluxesinHanumanlangurslivinginmulti-malegroups.In:PrimateMales:CausesandConsequencesofVariationinGroupCompositioned.P.M.Kappeler,pp.146-58.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Borries,C.,Launhardt,K.,Epplen,C.,Epplen,J.T.&Winkler,P.1999.MalesasinfantprotectorsinHanumanlangursPresbytisentelluslivinginmultimalegroups:defencepattern,paternity,andsexualbehaviour.BehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,350-6.Brockman,D.K.1999.ReproductivebehavioroffemalePropithecusverreauxiatBezaMahafaly,Madagascar.InternationalJournalofPrimatology,375-98.Chapais,B.1983.Reproductiveactivityinrelationtomaledominanceandthelikelihoodofovulationinrhesusmonkeys.BehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,215-28.Cheney,D.L.1983.Proximateandultimatefactorsrelatedtothedistributionofmalemigration.In:PrimateSocialRelationships:AnIntegratedApproach,ed.R.A.Hinde,pp.241-9.Oxford:BlackwellScientificPublications.Clutton-Brock,T.H.&Harvey,P.H.1976.Evolutionaryrulesandprimatesocieties.In:GrowingPointsinEthology,ed.P.P.G.Bateson&R.A.Hinde,pp.195-237.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Colmenares,F.&Gomendio,M.1988.Changesinfemalereproductiveconditionfollowingmaletake-oversinacolonyofhamadryasandhybridbaboons.FoliaPrimatologica157-74.Cords,M.1984.MatingpatternsandsocialstructureinredtailmonkeysCercopithecusascaniusZeitschriftfürTierpsychologie,313-29.-1988.Matingsystemsofforestguenons:apreliminaryreview.In:APrimateRadiation:EvolutionaryBiologyoftheAfricanGuenons,ed.A.Gautier-Hion,F.Bourliere,J.P.Gautier&J.Kingdon,pp.323-39.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Cowlishaw,G.&Dunbar,R.I.M.1991.Dominancerankandmatingsuccessinmaleprimates.AnimalBehaviour,1045-56.Cox,C.R.&LeBoeuf,B.J.1977.Femaleincitationofmalecompetition:amechanisminsexualselection.AmericanNaturalist111,317-35.Crockett,C.M.&Sekulic,R.1984.InfanticideinredhowlermonkeysAlouattaseniculus.In:Infanticide:ComparativeandEvolutionaryPerspectives,ed.G.Hausfater&S.B.Hrdy,pp.173-91.NewYork:AldinedeGruyter.Darwin,C.1876.Sexualselectioninrelationtomonkeys.Nature,18-9.Dixson,A.F.1983.Observationsontheevolutionandbehavioralsignificanceof‘sexualskin’infemaleprimates.AdvancesintheStudyofBehavior,63-106.-1998.PrimateSexuality:ComparativeStudiesoftheProsimiansMonkeysApesandHumanBeings.Oxford:OxfordUniversityPress.Dolhinow,P.J.1977.Normalmonkeys?AmericanScientist266.Fox,E.A.1998.ThefunctionoffemalematechoiceintheSumatranorangutan(Pongopygmaeusabelii).PhDthesis,DukeUniversity.Gowaty,P.A.1997.Sexualdialectics,sexualselection,andvariationinmatingbehavior.In:FeminismandEvolutionaryBiology:Boundaries,Intersections,andFrontiers,ed.P.A.Gowaty,pp.351-84.NewYork:Chapman&HallGoodall,J.1986.TheChimpanzeesofGombe.Cambridge,MA:HarvardUniversityPressGust,D.A.1994.Alpha-malesootymangabeysdifferentiatebetweenfemales’fertileandtheirpostconceptionmaximal 161 swellings.InternationalJournalofPrimatology,289-302.Gygax,L.1995.HidingbehaviouroflongtailedmacaquesMacacafascicularis.I.Theoreticalbackgroundanddataonmating.Ethology101,1-24.Hamilton,W.J.&Bulger,J.B.1990.Natalmalebaboonrankrisesandsuccessfulchallengestoresidentalphamales.BehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,357-62.Hammerstein,P.&Parker,G.A.1987.Sexualselection:gamesbetweenthesexes.In:SexualSelection:TestingtheAlternatives,ed.J.W.Bradbury&M.B.Andersson,pp.119-42.NewYork:JohnWiley.Harada,Y.&Iwasa,Y.1996.Femalematepreferencetomaximizepaternalcare:atwo-stepgame.AmericanNaturalist147,996-1027.Hausfater,G.&Hrdy,S.B.1984.Infanticide:ComparativeandEvolutionaryPerspectives.NewYork:AldinedeGruyter.Hayssen,V.,vanTienhoven,A.&vanTienhoven,A.1993.Asdell’sPatternsofMammalianReproduction:ACompendiumofSpecies-SpecificData.Ithaca,NY:ComstockPublishingAssociates.Heistermann,M.,Ziegler,T.,vanSchaik,C.P.,Launhardt,K.,Winkler,P.&Hodges,J.K.2001.Lossofoestrus,concealedovulationandpaternityconfusioninfree-rangingHanumanlangurs.ProceedingsoftheRoyalSocietyLondonSeriesB268,2445-51.Henzi,S.P.&Lucas,J.W.1980.Observationsontheinter-troopmovementofadultvervetmonkeysCercopithecusaethiopsFoliaPrimatologica,220-35.Hrdy,S.B.1974.Male-malecompetitionandinfanticideamongthelangursPresbytisentellusofAbu,Rajasthan.FoliaPrimatologica,19-58.-1979.Infanticideamonganimals:areview,classification,andexaminationoftheimplicationsforthereproductivestrategiesoffemales.EthologyandSociobiology,13-40.-1981.TheWomanthatNeverEvolved.Cambridge,MA:HarvardUniversityPress.-1997.RaisingDarwin’sconsciousness:femalesexualityandtheprehominidoriginsofpatriarchy.HumanNature,1-49.-2000a.Foreword.In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplicationsed.C.P.vanSchaik&C.H.Janson,pp.xi-xiv.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Hrdy,S.B.&Whitten,P.L.1987.Patterningofsexualactivity.In:PrimateSocieties,ed.B.B.Smuts,D.L.Cheney,R.M.Seyfarth,R.W.Wrangham&T.T.Struhsaker,pp.370-84.Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.Huffman,M.A.1987.ConsortintrusionandfemalematechoiceinJapanesemacaquesMacacafuscataEthology,221-34.-1992.InfluencesoffemalepartnerpreferenceonpotentialreproductiveoutcomeinJapanesemacaques.FoliaPrimatologica,77-88.Janson,C.H.1986.Thematingsystemasadeterminantofsocialevolutionincapuchinmonkeys(Cebus).In:ProceedingsoftheXInternationalCongressofPrimatology,ed.J.Else&P.C.Lee,pp.169-79.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Janson,C.H.&vanSchaik,C.P.2000.Thebehavioralecologyofinfanticidebymales.In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications,ed.C.P.vanSchaik&C.H.Janson,pp.469-94.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Jones,C.B.1985.Reproductivepatternsinmantledhowlermonkeys:estrus,matechoice,andcopulation.Primates130-42.Kappeler,P.M.1991.Patternsofsexualdimorphisminbodyweightamongprosimianprimates.FoliaPrimatologica132-46.-1993.Femaledominanceinprimatesandothermammals.In:PerspectivesinEthology,Vol.10ed.P.P.G.Bateson,N.Thompson&P.H.Klopfer,143-58.NewYork:PlenumPress.Kummer,H.1995.InQuestoftheSacredBaboon:AScientist’sJourney.Princeton,NJ:PrincetonUniversityPress.Manson,J.H.1992.MeasuringfemalematechoiceinCayoSantiagorhesusmacaques.AnimalBehaviour,405-16.-1994.Maleaggression:acostoffemalematechoiceinCayoSantiagorhesusmacaques.AnimalBehaviour,473-5.-1995.Primateconsortships:acriticalreview.CurrentAnthropology,353-74.Manson,J.H.,Perry,S.&Parish,A.R.1997.Nonconceptivesexualbehaviorinbonobosandcapuchins.InternationalJournalofPrimatology,767-86.Martin,R.D.1992.Femalecyclesinrelationtopaternityinprimatesocieties.In:PaternityinPrimates:GeneticTestsandTheories,ed.R.D.Martin,A.F.Dixson&E.J.Wickings,pp.238-74.Basel:Karger.Matsumoto-Oda,A.&Oda,R.1998.Changesintheactivitybudgetofcyclingfemalechimpanzees.AmericanJournalofPrimatology,157-66.Mitani,J.C.1985.MatingbehaviourofmaleorangutansintheKutaiGameReserve,Indonesia.AnimalBehaviour,392-402.Nieuwenhuijsen,K.,deNeef,K.J.&Slob,A.K.1986.Sexualbehaviourduringovariancycles,pregnancyandlactationingroup-livingstumptailmacaquesMacacaarctoidesHumanReproduction,159-69.Noë,R.&Sluijter,A.A.1990.Reproductivetacticsofmalesavannababoons.Behaviour113,117-70.Nunn,C.L.1999.Theevolutionofexaggeratedsexualswellingsinprimatesandthegradedsignalhypothesis.AnimalBehaviour,229-46.Nunn,C.L.&vanSchaik,C.P.2000.Socialevolutioninprimates:therelativerolesofecologyandintersexualconflict.In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications,ed.C.P.vanSchaik&C.H.Janson,pp.388-420.Cambridge:Cambridge 162 UniversityPress.O’Connell,S.&Cowlishaw,G.1994.Infanticideavoidance,spermcompetitionandmatechoice:thefunctionofcopulationcallsinfemalebaboons.AnimalBehaviour,687-94.Oda,R.&Masataka,N.1995.FunctionofcopulatoryvocalizationsinmatechoicebyfemalesofJapanesemacaquesMacacafuscataFoliaPrimatologica,132-9.Parmigiani,S.&vomSaal,F.S.(eds.)1994.InfanticideandParentalCare.NewYork:Harwood.Paul,A.1989.DeterminantsofmalematingsuccessinalargegroupofBarbarymacaquesMacacasylvanusatAffenbergSalem.Primates,461-76.-1997.Breedingseasonalityaffectstheassociationbetweendominanceandreproductivesuccessinnon-humanmaleprimates.FoliaPrimatologica,344-9.Paul,A.,Preuschoft,S.&vanSchaik,C.P.2000.Theothersideofthecoin:infanticideandtheevolutionofaffiliativemale-infantinteractionsinOldWorldprimates.In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications,ed.C.P.vanSchaik&C.H.Janson,pp.269-92.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Pereira,M.E.&Weiss,M.L.1991.Femalematechoice,malemigration,andthethreatofinfanticideinringtailedlemurs.BehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,141-52.Perrigo,G.&vomSaal,F.S.1994.Behavioralcyclesandtheneuraltimingofinfanticideandparentalbehaviorinmalehousemice.In:InfanticideandParentalCare,ed.S.Parmigiani&F.S.vomSaal,pp.365-96.Chur,Switzerland:HarwoodAcademicPublishers.Perry,S.1998.Male-malesocialrelationshipsinwildwhite-facedcapuchins,CebuscapucinusBehaviour135,139-72.Pizzari,T.&Birkhead,T.R.2000.Femaleferalfowlejectspermofsubdominantmales.Nature405,787-9.Resnik,M.D.1987.Choices:AnIntroductiontoDecisionTheoryMinneapolis:UniversityofMinnesotaPress.Richard,A.1992.Aggressivecompetitionbetweenmales,female-controlledpolygynyandsexualmonomorphisminaMalagasyprimate,Propithecusverreauxi.JournalofHumanEvolution,395-406.Robinson,J.G.1988.Demographyandgroupstructureinwedge-cappedcapuchinmonkeys,CebusolivaceusBehaviour202-32.Samuels,A.,Silk,J.B.&Rodman,P.S.1984.ChangesinthedominancerankandreproductivebehaviourofmalebonnetmacaquesMacacaradiataAnimalBehaviour,994-1003.Sauther,M.L.1991.Reproductivebehavioroffree-rangingLemurcattaatBezaMahafalySpecialReserve,Madagascar.AmericanJournalofPhysicalAnthropology,463-77.Schürmann,C.L.&vanHooff,J.A.R.A.M.1986.Reproductivestrategiesoftheorang-utan:newdataandareconsiderationofexistingsociosexualmodels.InternationalJournalofPrimatology,265-87.Semple,S.&McComb,K.2000.Perceptionoffemalereproductivestatefromvocalcuesinamammalspecies.ProceedingsoftheRoyalSocietyofLondon,SeriesB707-12.Small,M.F.1993.FemaleChoices:SexualBehaviorofFemalePrimates.Ithaca:CornellUniversityPress.Smith,D.G.1994.MaledominanceandreproductivesuccessinacaptivegroupofrhesusmacaquesMacacamulattaBehaviour129,225-42.Smuts,B.B.1987.Gender,aggression,andinfluence.In:PrimateSocieties,ed.B.B.Smuts,D.L.Cheney,R.M.Seyfarth,R.W.Wrangham&T.T.Struhsaker,pp.400-12.Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.Smuts,B.B.&Smuts,R.W.1993.Maleaggressionandsexualcoercionoffemalesinnonhumanprimatesandothermammals:evidenceandtheoreticalimplications.AdvancesintheStudyofBehavior,1-63.Soltis,J.1999.Measuringmale-femalerelationshipsduringthematingseasoninwildJapanesemacaquesMacacafuscatayakuiPrimates,453-67.Soltis,J.,Thomsen,R.,Matsubayashi,K.&Takenaka,O.2000.Infanticidebyresidentmalesandfemalecounter-strategiesinwildJapanesemacaquesMacacafuscataBehavioralEcologyandSociobiology,195-202.Sommer,V.1994.InfanticideamongthelangursofJodhpur:testingthesexualselectionhypothesiswithalong-termrecord.In:InfanticideandParentalCare,ed.S.Parmigiani&F.S.vomSaal,pp.155-98.London:HarwoodAcademicPublishers.Sommer,V.,Srivastava,A.&Borries,C.1992.Cycles,sexuality,andconceptioninfree-ranginglangurs.AmericanJournalofPrimatology,1-28.Steenbeek,R.1996.WhatamalelessgroupcantellusabouttheconstraintsonfemaletransferinThomas’slangursPresbytisthomasiFoliaPrimatologica,169-81.Stein,D.M.1984.TheSociobiologyofInfantandAdultMaleBaboons.Norwood,NJ:Ablex.Stephens,D.W.&Krebs,J.R.1986.ForagingTheory.Princeton,NJ:PrincetonUniversityPress.Strier,K.B.1994.Mythofthetypicalprimate.YearbookofPhysicalAnthropology,233-71.Struhsaker,T.T.&Leland,L.1985.Infanticideinapatrilinealsocietyofredcolobusmonkeys.ZeitschriftfürTierpsychologie,89-132.Sugiyama,Y.1966.AnartificialsocialchangeinahanumanlangurtroopPresbytisentellusPrimates,41-72.Sussman,R.W.,Cheverud,J.M.&Bartlett,T.Q.1995.Infantkilingasanevolutionarystrategy:realityormyth?EvolutionaryAnthropology,149-51.Swedell,L.2000.Twotakeoversinwildhamadryasbaboons.FoliaPrimatologica,169-72. 163 Symons,D.1982.Anotherwomanthatneverexisted.QuarterlyReviewofBiology,297-300.Takahata,Y.,Sprague,D.S.,Suzuki,S.&Okayasy,N.1994.Femalecompetition,co-existence,andthematingstructureofwildJapanesemacaquesonYakushimaisland,Japan.In:AnimalSocieties:IndividualsInteractionsandOrganisationed.P.J.Jarman&A.Rossiter,pp.163-79.Kyoto:KyotoUniversityPress.Tutin,C.E.G.1979.Responsesofchimpanzeestocopulation,withspecialreferencetointerferencebyimmatureindividuals.AnimalBehaviour,845-54.vanNoordwijk,M.A.1985.SexualbehaviourofSumatranlong-tailedmacaquesMacacafascicularisZeitschriftfürTierpsychologie,277-96.vanNoordwijk,M.A.&vanSchaik,C.P.1985.Malemigrationandrankacquisitioninwildlong-tailedmacaques(MacacafascicularisAnimalBehaviour,849-61.-1988.MalecareersinSumatranlong-tailedmacaquesMacacafascicularisBehaviour107,24-43.-2000.Reproductivepatternsineutherianmammals:adaptationsagainstinfanticide?In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications,ed.C.P.vanSchaik&J.H.Janson,pp.322-60.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.-2001.Careermoves:transferandrankchallengedecisionsbymalelong-tailedmacaques.Behaviour,359-95.vanSchaik,C.P.1996.Socialevolutioninprimates:theroleofecologicalfactorsandmalebehaviour.ProceedingsoftheBritishAcademy,9-31.-2000a.Infanticidebymaleprimates:thesexualselectionhypothesisrevisited.In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications,ed.C.P.vanSchaik&C.H.Janson,pp.27-60.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.-2000b.Vulnerabilitytoinfanticide:patternsamongmammals.In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications,ed.C.P.vanSchaik&C.H.Janson,pp.61-71.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.vanSchaik,C.P.&Dunbar,R.I.M.1990.Theevolutionofmonogamyinlargeprimates:anewhypothesisandsomecrucialtests.Behaviour115,30-62.vanSchaik,C.P.&Janson,C.H.(eds.)2000.InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPressvanSchaik,C.P.&Kappeler,P.M.1997.Infanticideriskandtheevolutionofmale-femaleassociationinprimates.ProceedingsoftheRoyalSocietyLondonSeriesB264,1687-94.vanSchaik,C.P.,vanNoordwijk,M.A.&Nunn,C.L.1999.Sexandsocialevolutioninprimates.In:ComparativePrimateSocioecology,ed.P.C.Lee,pp.204-40.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.vanSchaik,C.P.,Hodges,J.K.&Nunn,C.L.2000.Paternityconfusionandtheovariancyclesoffemaleprimates.In:InfanticidebyMalesandItsImplications,ed.C.P.vanSchaik&C.H.Janson,pp.361-87.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.Virgin,C.E.&Sapolsky,R.M.1997.Stylesofmalesocialbehaviorandtheirendocrinecorrelatesamonglow-rankingbaboons.AmericanJournalofPrimatology,25-39.vomSaal,F.S.1984.Proximateandultimatecausesofinfanticideandparentalbehaviorinmalehousemice.In:Infanticide:ComparativeandEvolutionaryPerspectives,ed.G.Hausfater&S.B.Hrdy,pp.401-24.NewYork:AldinedeGruyter.Wallis,J.&Bettinger,T.1993.Sexualactivityinwildchimpanzees:acomparisonofcyclic,pregnant,andlactatingfemaleswithfullanogenitalswellings.AmericanJournalofPrimatology,353.Watts,D.P.1991.Mountaingorillareproductionandsexualbehavior.AmericanJournalofPrimatology,211-26.Wrangham,R.W.1979.Ontheevolutionofapesocialsystems.SocialSciencesInformation,334-68.Zhao,Q.-K.1993.SexualbehavioroftibetanmacaquesatatMt.Emei,China.Primates,431-44.Zinner,D.&Deschner,T.2000.Sexualswellingsinfemalehamadryasbaboonsaftermaletake-over:‘deceptive’swellingsasapossiblefemalecounter-strategyagainstinfanticide.AmericanJournalofPrimatology,157-68.