/
Human Language, Segmental distribution of speech sounds in Human Language, Segmental distribution of speech sounds in

Human Language, Segmental distribution of speech sounds in - PowerPoint Presentation

pasty-toler
pasty-toler . @pasty-toler
Follow
463 views
Uploaded On 2016-06-17

Human Language, Segmental distribution of speech sounds in - PPT Presentation

th century Modern Phonological paradigms and theories DrHemanga Dutta Assistant Professor Department of Linguistics and Contemporary English School of Language Sciences The English and Foreign languages University EFLU ID: 365164

onset cluster sonority consonant cluster onset consonant sonority segments assamese sounds nasal ons stop liquids word initial language syllable

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Presentation The PPT/PDF document "Human Language, Segmental distribution o..." is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

Slide1

Human Language, Segmental distribution of speech sounds in prosodic domain: Evidence from 20th century Modern Phonological paradigms and theories

Dr.Hemanga

Dutta

Assistant Professor, Department of Linguistics and Contemporary English, School of Language Sciences

The English and Foreign languages University, (EFLU)

Hyderbad

: 500007

Mail id: hemangadutta1@gmail.comSlide2

Language from various perspectives: History, Philosophy, Biology and Sociology

What is Language?

Language , pre structural paradigm and philological concerns

Language and Structural principles of Saussure and B.F. Skinner

Language,

Cognitivism

, Chomsky and generative paradigm

Language, Communication and Functional prerequisites to competence modelSlide3

Phonetic underpinnings of Human linguistic mechanism

Phonetics: three main components

– Articulatory, Acoustic and Auditory

Articulatory

phonetic paradigms: Air stream mechanism, Process of Phonation, Oro nasal process

Speech organs

Manner of articulation: Plosive, Fricative, Affricate, Laterals, Approximants, Nasals

Place of articulation: Bilabial,

Labio

dental, Dental, Alveolar, Post alveolar, Retroflex, Velar, Uvular, Glottal etc.Slide4

Phonology and Generative paradigm

Phonology as organization of sounds in to patterns

Phonology as

F

unctional Phonetics

Difference between Phonetics and Phonology

Integration model between Phonetics and Phonology

Classical Phonemic model: Phoneme, Allophones, Contrastive and Complementary Distribution and Free variationSlide5

Phonemic analysis of LambadaContrastive Distribution:

Contrastive distribution refers to a process in which two phones can occur in identical environment and the substitution of one sound with another creates a semantic difference.

/p/, /b/

[

pɑːɳi

] - water

[

bɑːɳi

] broom

[

pa:p

] sin

[

baːp

] father

Here /p/ is substituted by the phoneme /b/.Slide6

Free variationTwo phonetically identical sounds are always in free variation. Where the one occurs the other can occur without changing the meaning.

/

/, /ð/

[

kaːgədʒ

] -paper

[

kaːgəð

]-paper

Here /

/ is substituted by the phoneme /d/ which does not bring change in meaning.

/k/, /θ/

[

tʃoːkra

ː] -boy

[

tʃoːθra

ː] -boySlide7

Complementary distributionWhen two sounds occur in mutually exclusive environment they are called allophones according to the principle of complementary distribution.The distribution of clear /l/ and dark /

/ in English

kli:n

‘clean’

lait

‘light’ lit ‘little’Slide8

Distinctive feature theory and the issue of Binarism

Distinctive feature theory the essence of which can be traced back to

Panini’s

Pratyaharas

, was first formalised by the Prague

Structuralists

although it attained its prominence in the hands of Chomsky and Halle in 1968 with the publication of SPE.

Motivations:

All segments must be

characterizeable

in terms of unique combination of features. They can show the segment inventories of the language.

They should be able to show the segmental contrast in the world languages.

The segments must be characterizable in phonetic terms. This has led to the requirement of the Natural ness condition (Postal 1968), according to which distinctive features must have a phonetic underpinnings.Slide9

Major class features

[+/-

sonorant

]: [+

sonorant

] segments are produced with a constriction in the vocal tract which allows an air pressure behind it and in front of it to be relatively equal, while it is not the case for [-

sonorant

] segments. [+

sonorant

] segments include vowels, glides, liquids and nasals while [-

sonorant

] are plosives, fricatives, affricates and laryngeal segments.

[+/- consonantal] : [+consonantal] segments have a constriction somewhere along the centre line in the vocal tract which is at least as narrow as that required for a fricative. [-cons] lacks such constriction. [+cons] segments are plosives, affricates, fricatives, liquids and nasals. [-cons] sounds are vowels, glides.

[+/-approximant]: [+approx] segments allow frictional escape of air (

Ladefoged

1971, Clements 1989). Vowels and non nasal sounds like laterals and

rhotics

are [+approx].

[+/- syllabic]: segments which can become nucleus of a syllable are [+syllabic].Slide10

Laryngeal features:Laryngeal features imply the glottal properties of the segments.[+/-voice]

[+/-spread glottis]: [+

s.g

] segments are characterized by a vocal cord configuration that produces audible glottal friction. Aspirated segments are [+

s.g

].Slide11

Manner features: Manner features are based on the type of constriction or the manner of articulation.

[+/-continuant] : continuant sounds are produced when the primary constriction in the vocal tract does not block the air flow. In the case of non continuant sounds the air flow through the mouth is stopped. Plosives, affricates, nasal s, laterals are [-cont] whereas vowels and fricatives are [+cont].

[+/-nasal]

[+/-lateral]

 Slide12

Place features[LABIAL] : segments are articulated with the lips and in the case of vowels with lips rounding. LABIALS may be specified for [+/round].

[CORONAL]: These segments are articulated with a raised tip or blade of the tongue. t, d, s etc are [+coronal] where as p, b, m etc are [-coronal]. [CORONAL] segments are further specifies for the features [+/- anterior] and [+/-distributed]

[+/-anterior] sounds are articulated in front of the

palato

alveolar region of the mouth..

[+/-distributed] sounds are those during the production of which the articulatory constrictions extends for a considerable distance along the direction of the air flow. Apical consonants are [-distributed] whereas laminal consonants are [+distributed].

[DORSAL] sounds are articulated with dorsum. Velars and uvulars are [+dorsal].

[Radical] also known as [Pharyngeal] articulated with root of the tongueSlide13

Features relating to the body of the tongue:

[+/-high]: when the body of the tongue is raised above the neutral position, the resultant sound is high.

[+/-low]: Low sounds are produced when the body of the tongue is lowered below the neutral position.

[+/-back] : Retraction of the back of the tongue from the neutral position during the production of a sound makes it a back sound.

[+/-tense]: [+tense] vowels are produced with considerable muscular effort.Slide14

Acoustic feature[+ /-strident] : [+strident] sounds are marked acoustically by greater noise than non strident sounds. Fricatives are [+strident] sounds.Slide15

DF and some questionsDo DFs

characterizing a language from part of a universal inventory?

Are the correlates of DF primarily articulatory, acoustic or both?

Should

DFs

be binary or not?

DF is conceived as a universal inventory capable of describing the phonology of any language. One standard argument in favour of universalism is that recurrent and finite nature of dimensions that phonologists refer to in the analysis of phonological systems, whether in terms of rules,

phonotactic

statements or in system inventories.

Although

DFs

are more abstract than their implementations, they are grounded in phonetics.Slide16

Assamese phonotactics and initial onset consonant cluster: sonority, optimality and binding principles

Stop + Liquid

/pr/

pran

‘life’

prem ‘love’

/pl/

plawon

‘shower’

plawito

‘filled with water’

/

br

/

brɒhmandɒ

‘universe’

/

bʰr

/

bʰrɒm

‘illusion’

/

bl

/

blauz

‘blouse’

/tr/

troyudox

‘thirteenth’

tritiyo

‘third’

/

dr

/

drɒbjɒ

‘substance’

dristi

‘vision’

/

kr

/

krɒm

‘serial’

krɒmannoye

‘gradually’

/

kl

/

klantɒ

‘tired’

/gr/

grismɒ

‘summer’

grɒhɒn

‘eclipse’

/

gl

/

glani

‘repentance’

/

gʰr

/

gʰran

‘smell’

/

tj

/

tjag

‘sacrifice’

/

gj

/

gjan

‘knowledge’

The

unpermitted

initial consonant cluster comprising of stop and liquid are */

tl

/ and */dl/ although they conform to the principle of sonority hierarchy.Slide17

Nasal +Liquid/mr/

mrityu

‘death’

mrigo

‘deer’

/ml/

mlan

‘pale’

/nr/

nritj

ɔ

‘dance’

/

nj

/

njai

‘justice’

In Assamese

phonotactics

the alveolar and velar nasal can not constitute the cluster with liquid in initial onset position of a syllable. So the prohibited patterns are */

nl

/, */

ŋr

/ and */

ŋl

/.Slide18

Fricative + Liquid/sr/

srɒm

‘labour’

sristi

‘creation’

sriŋɒ

‘peak’

/hr/

hridɒi

‘heart’

In Assamese

phonotactics

there are only two fricatives /s/ and /h/ which can function as the initial member of a cluster comprising of fricative and liquid.

The prohibited patterns are:

*/

zr

/, */

zl

/, */hl/Slide19

Fricative + Stop/ st/

stɒmbʰɒ

‘pillar’

stɒn

‘breast’

stʊti

‘prayer’

/

st

ʰ/

stʰan

‘place’

stʰanijɒ

‘local’

/sp/

spɒrxɒ

‘touch’

spriha

‘desire’

/spʰ/

spʰʊliŋɒ

‘ashes’

/

sk

/

skrin

‘screen’

/

sk

ʰ/

skʰɒlɒn

‘degradation’

This is the special property of the Assamese post alveolar fricative /s/ that can function as the initial member of a consonant cluster followed by voiceless stops. But it cannot make cluster with the voiced plosives of Assamese. So, the prohibited shapes in Assamese consonant cluster

phonotactics

are: */

sd

/, */

sb

/, */sg/Slide20

Fricative + Nasal/sm/

smɒxam

‘graveyard’

/

sn

/

snigdʰɒ

‘calm’

snan

‘bathe’

The post alveolar fricative /s/ can form consonant cluster with bilabial and alveolar nasal as initial member of the consonant cluster. The velar nasal /ŋ/ never becomes the member of any consonant cluster. What is interesting to note that other

ficative

sounds /z/ and /h/ are not allowed in Assamese

phonotactics

to be the members of a consonant cluster with nasals. Hence the prohibited consonant clusters are : */

/, */

zm

/, */

zn

/,*/

/, */

hm

/, */

hn

/, */

/Slide21

Word initial clusters such as /pl, gl, pr, tr, dr, gr/ are found but such as */

lp

,

rt

/, etc, where the sonority relations are reversed, are not present in the

phonotactics

of Assamese. So, it can be argued that sonority constraints play an important role in the patterning of segmental distribution in the

phonotactics

of a language. Slide22

This hierarchy of onsets yield markedness constraints on onset sonority, as proposed in de Lacy (2001)*ONS/L >> *ONS/N>>*ONS/O

In

Assmese

, the ban on liquids as the first member in word initial cluster can be represented by using the constraints in OT framework in the following way:

*ONS-L >> IDENT-I0 [approx] insures that liquid onsets will be eliminated.

But word initial syllables in Assamese are allowed to begin with nasal consonants. It can be shown by the following ranking of the OT constraints:

IDENT-IO [approx] >>*ONS/NSlide23

Moreover, what is seen from Assamese onset consonant cluster is that the second member of the cluster agrees in terms of feature [voice] with the first member of the cluster. It can be formulated in the following fashion in the form of a constraint: Given a consonant cluster C1C2, if C1 is voiced, then C2 must be. Slide24

Word initial onset cluster in Assamese can be formed by combining either stop + liquid or nasal+liquid. But liquid does not have the potential to be the initial member of the onset cluster. As, for instance the following are not the onset cluster in Assamese permitted by the

phonotactic

constraints in Assamese: *

rp

, *

rt

, *

lp

, *

rk

, *

rm

, *lm

The asymmetry here is motivated by the Complexity Condition: nasals containing three elements have priority over liquids, which contain two or sometimes only one element. Thus if we argue in lines of Harris (1990) it is to be noted that sonority is structurally encoded where a correlation can be established between sonority and segmental complexity. As we have seen in the above illustration of Assamese data plosives govern liquids on the ground that plosives are more complex than liquids in terms of internal components involved in representation. According to Harris (1990) the more

sonorant

a segment, the less complex its representation. But Rice (1992) has argued that greater sonority implies greater complexity. Slide25

Harris (1990) interpretation: Coronal stop Coronal nasal Coronal lateral

x

x

x

? Rᵒ

Rᵒ

Rᵒ ?ᵒ

?ᵒ

hᵒ N+

H-

Here, ? = occlusion ; Rᵒ = coronal; hᵒ= noise and H- = stiff vocal cords

Representation of coronal stop, nasal and lateral in the framework of Harris(1990)Slide26

Rice (1992) interpretation: ROOT ROOT

ROOT

SL AF SL AF SL AF

 

Place

Place

SV Place SV

 

Lateral

Here, SL=

Supralaryngeal

; AF= Air Flow; SV=

Sonorant

Voice

Representation of coronal stop, nasal and lateral in the framework of Rice(1992)Slide27

sonority profile within an onset is met only if the second consonant has more SV structure than the first. Stop Lateral

ROOT

ROOT

SL

SL

SVSlide28

Margin hierarchy approach and consonant cluster arrangment Margin hierarchy of Prince and

Smolensky

(1993), gives preference to segments of low sonority. This constraint is applicable to singleton onsets or to the first member of an onset cluster and it is known as M1 hierarchy. In the same way, the M2 hierarchy applies both to the second member of an onset and a singleton coda. It differs from M1 hierarchy in the sense that it gives preference to consonants of high sonority.

M1 hierarchy (preference given to consonants of low sonority)

*M1/r >> *M1/ l >> *M1/

Nas

>> *M1/obs

 M2 hierarchy (preference given to consonants of high sonority)

*M2/

obs

>> *M2/ Nasal >> *M2/l >> *M2/rSlide29

The *ONSET/X subhierarchy assumed here is shown below:*ONS/GLIDE>>*ONS/RHOTIC>>*ONS/LATERAL>>*ONS/NASAL>>*ONS/VOICED

OBST>>*ONS/VCLSOBST

Smith (2003) claims what is appealing about the *ONSET/X

subhierarchy

is that, as this sub hierarchy is based on the sonority scale and the perceptual preference for low sonority onsets, it is functionally grounded. Slide30

This preference for low sonority onsets is assigned functional motivation too. The auditory system is particularly sensitive to rapid changes in spectral patterns (Stevens 1989; Ohala 1992;

Delgutte

1997; Warner 1998).

Delgutte

(1997) claims that a low sonority onset is preferred because it is more distinct from the syllable nucleus than a high sonority onset would be. Slide31

Assamese phonotactics, Binding principle and OCP constraint:

In Assamese

phonotactics

we have observed that some clusters are not permitted to occur in word initial position. The cluster types are */

tl

/, */dl/, */

sl

/, */

zr

/, */

zl

/, etc. A bound consonant contains dependent structure. i.e. identical Place structure to the consonant that binds it or no Place structure.

Binding is not allowed within an onset (i.e. a consonant can not be syllabified into an onset if it shares place with the adjacent syllabified consonant.

A consonant must be bound for Place

heterosyllabically

(i.e. a consonant may be syllabified into a rhyme if it is non distinct in Place from the following onset). (parametric).Slide32

Binding: /p/ and /l/ differ in place of articulation, and thus /l/ is not bound. Hence, the syllabification is well formed. But the prohibited consonant cluster such as */dl/ can be accounted for in the light of binding principles.Slide33

s as Branching onset (Carlisle 1988, Major 1996,2001, Ohala 1999)

σ

ONS R

X X X

s t oSlide34

s as Complex segment (Selkirk 1982, Lamontagne 1993, Van de Weijer

1996)

  σ

ONS R

 

X

X

 

s t oSlide35

s as Adjunct (Barlow 2001, Barlow and Dinnnsen 1998, Kaye 1989,

Kenstowicz

1994)

σ

ONS R

X X X

s t oSlide36

s as Extrasyllabic (Appendix) (Goad and Rose 2004, Fikkert

1994, Levin 1985,

Giegerich

1992)

σ

 

ONS R

X

X

 

s t oSlide37

In opposition to the branching onset approach the other alternative approaches ranging from complex segment approach to extrasyllabic approach aim at eliminating the SSP violations via the assignment of abstract representations. Slide38

Moreover, sonority sequencing constraints such as syllable contact treats s-obstruent clusters differently from obstruent- sonorant clusters. s-obstruent clusters have falling sonority; hence epenthesis at the edge is possible and preferred. Consider the Assamese example:

skul

>

is.kul

skrin

>

is.krin

In the above example of loan word in Assamese, epenthesis occurs at the edge. The crucial assumption here is that the default site of epenthesis in loan words is at the edge.Slide39

It is observed that edge epenthesis violates NO CODA and ONSET whereas the dispreferred internal epenthesis satisfies NO CODA, ONSET and SYLLABLE CONTACT. In order to justify this edge epenthesis

Gouskova

(2001) talks about the constraint CONTIGUITYSlide40

CONTIGUITY: elements adjacent in the input must be adjacent in the output. This constraint ensures edge epenthesis when SYLLABLE CONTACT is not at stake. SYLLABLE CONTACT implies sonority must not rise across a syllable boundary (Davis 1998, Hooper 1976, Murray and

Vennemann

1983,

Vennemannn

1988).

/

skul

/ *COMPLEX DEP SYLL CONTACT CONTIGUITY

is.kul

*

 

si.kul

* *!

Slide41

In the above tableau SYLLABLE CONTACT is not violated and hence, CONTIGUITY ensures that edge epenthesis is optimal. So from this tableau it is revealed that SYLLABLE CONTACT determines the epenthesis site: at the edge for falling sonority clusters and internal epenthesis for rising sonority clusters. This outcome can be expected as long as SYLLABLE CONTACT is ranked above CONTIGUITY, although its ranking in relation to DEP is not crucial. This pattern is termed as the Emergence of the Unmarked effect.Slide42

Gemination Gemination as a process triggered by liquids and semivowels:

In this section I am proposing a problem that in Sanskrit gemination of obstruents is triggered by the following liquids and glides such as

j,r,l,ʋ

but not other segments such as nasals and obstruents.

jət̪rə

-

jət̪.t̪rə

(here)

t̪ət̪rə

-

t̪ət̪.t̪rə

(there)

ʃukrə

-

ʃuk.krə

(

venus

)

bʰəd̪rə

-

bʰəd̪.d̪rə

(descent)

nepət̪ʰjə

-

nepət̪.t̪ʰjə

(back stage)

karjə

-

kar.rjə

(work)

pəd̪jə

-

pəd̪.d̪jə

(poetry)

pəkʋə

-

pək.kʋə

(ripe)

sət̪ʋə

-

sət̪.t̪ʋə

(white)

t̪ət̪ʋə

-

t̪ət̪.t̪ʋə

(theory)

ʃuklə

-

ʃuk.klə (bright)Slide43

The reason behind the motivation for the process of gemination triggered by glides and liquids can be supported with cross linguistic observation. For convenience, consider the internal representation of a word ‘

sətjə

’ (truth):

This internal representation can be made in two ways:

If we consider word internal stop as a better candidate for the coda position the representation will be as follows

 

σ

σ

s ə t̪ . j ə

Figure : Representation of word internal stop in coda position Slide44

In contrast, stop and liquids form well formed consonant cluster cross linguistically. From this perspective, the following will be the representation:

 

σ

σ

 

s ə . t̪ j ə

 

Figure No 1/E: Representation of word internal stop in the onset position forming a cluster with following liquid

From the above representations it is clear that either coda condition or well formed onset cluster condition will be fulfilled. But from cross linguistic evidence it is seen that alveolar and velar stops function not only as better candidates for the coda position in a word but also form well formed consonant cluster with liquids and glides.Slide45

The fulfilment of these two conditions lies in the process of gemination of the stops as shown in the following representation:

σ

 

s ə t̪ . t̪ j ə

Figure No 1/F: Representation of word internal stop both in the coda and the onset position

But, what is interesting to note in this context that other sounds apart from these liquids and semi vowels do not have the potential to trigger gemination to the previous obstruent. Slide46

As, for illustration consider the following instances:(5)

santi

- *

san.nti

ʃəbdə

- *

ʃəb.bdə

məndə

- *

mən.ndə

atma

- *

at.tma

The motivation hidden behind the non occurrence of the process of gemination of the obstruents followed by nasals and obstruents can be assigned to the fact that obstruent and nasals do not serve as well formed consonant cluster. Hence, it can be argued that glides and liquids have the special property of being appropriate coda as well as a member in the onset consonant cluster resulting in the process of gemination. In addition, another generalization can be observed from the above process of gemination that the asymmetry in phonological processes can be attributed to the segmental properties and cross linguistic well

formedness

conditions.Slide47

Conclusion

segmental property inherent in a sound and cross linguistic well formed conditions also play a significant role in the triggering of certain phonological processes.

Behind the patterning of consonant cluster in a specific manner the notion of phonological strength and

phonotactic

patterns can

be realized.

 Slide48

THANK YOU