/
Reductionism and Complex Systems Science: Implications for Reductionism and Complex Systems Science: Implications for

Reductionism and Complex Systems Science: Implications for - PowerPoint Presentation

phoebe-click
phoebe-click . @phoebe-click
Follow
447 views
Uploaded On 2016-06-30

Reductionism and Complex Systems Science: Implications for - PPT Presentation

David G Schlundt PhD Associate Professor of Psychology CRC Research Skills January 20 2011 Overview NIH party line on translation research Problems with the party line Reductionism in modern science ID: 383593

research obesity bmi trends obesity research trends bmi data person lbs overweight adults brfss systems science basic health reductionism

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Presentation The PPT/PDF document "Reductionism and Complex Systems Science..." is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

Slide1

Reductionism and Complex Systems Science: Implications for Translation Research in the Health and Behavioral Sciences

David G.

Schlundt

, Ph.D.

Associate Professor of Psychology

CRC Research Skills

January 20, 2011Slide2

Overview

NIH party line on translation research

Problems with the party line

Reductionism in modern science

Problems with reductionism

Complex systems science as an alternative

Problems with complex systems science

Examining the obesity epidemic as a real-life exemplar

Integrating scientific approaches

Implications for basic and applied research on obesitySlide3

What is Translation Research?

Problem:

basic research findings take years or decades to find their way into evidence-based practice

Problem:

Landmark clinical trials take years or decades to find their way into evidence-based practice

Problem:

The investment in basic research has not resulted in a corresponding improvement in health care delivery

Goal:

Translate the discoveries of basic scientific research into population level gains in healthSlide4

NIH Road Map

New Pathways to Discovery

- unravel the complexity of biologic systems and their regulation

Research Teams of the Future

– break down the barriers to interdisciplinary and

transdisciplinary

research

Re-engineering the Clinical Research Enterprise

– bring more scientists into clinical research

Solution:

Clinical Science Translation Awards (CTSA) – infrastructure to support clinical and translation research at academic institutionsSlide5

The T’s of translation

T1 – from bench to bedside

Taking basic biological sciences and using them to create useful diagnostic tests, drugs, and therapies

T2 – from bedside to community

Moving clinical research findings into evidence-based practice and looking at the impact on the public’s health

These definitions:

Were created by the basic scientists who run the NIH research enterprise

Imagine a one-way flow of knowledge from basic research to improved health care

Over simplify what is a complicated problem (how to improve human health)Slide6

Problems with the T1-T2 vision

The amount of resources at the NIH continues to be disproportionately allocated for basic research

The basic scientists in charge have underestimated the difficulty and amount of time required to plan and execute translation research studies

The clinical relevance of basic research findings is overestimated

Translation research proposals are too often reviewed by basic scientists who review translation studies using their basic research framework

Much greater improvement in population health could be achieved by improving current health care delivery – based standards of care that are not implemented

Much greater improvement in population health could be achieved through health care reformSlide7

Meta Scientific Models

There are assumptions and frameworks behind the practice of science that drive the questions, the methodologies, and the development of new knowledge

Philosophical Reductionism

Offshoot of materialist philosophy

Idea that one science (biology) can be reduced to the principals of another science (chemistry)

Drive to find the most basic explanation

There is potentially a single, underlying physical science that explains everything

Methodological Reductionism

The best scientific explanations come from breaking problems into their most fundamental elements

Goal of science is to identify, isolate, and study basic causal mechanisms

Approach is to create experiments in which only one parameter is allowed to vary so that its causal effect can be isolated

Goal is to develop mechanistic explanationsSlide8

Reductionism in Action

Much “basic” research follows a reductionist framework in biological and behavioral sciences

Reductionism

Leads to increasing specialization

Leads to problems being broken down into ever smaller problems

Leads to a rapidly expanding base of knowledge in which the pieces are largely disconnected from each other

Leads to new technologies and methodologies for achieving tighter and tighter control

of ever smaller processes

Even when the rationale for the research is an important clinical problem (e.g., diabetes, depression, schizophrenia), the research itself ends up isolating only a small piece of the problem and studying it out of context Slide9

Reductionism Impedes Clinical Discoveries

Reductionism is not the most efficient way to improve the physical and mental health of populations of human beings

Most “breakthroughs” in basic health and neuroscience do not lead to new diagnostic or treatment approaches

The overspecialization of disciplines makes it difficult for any one scientist to pull together enough basic knowledge to create meaningful new diagnostics or interventions

Funding of basic science does not encourage interdisciplinary or

transdisciplinary

cooperation needed to create clinical applicationsSlide10

Unintended consequences of reductionism

In reductionism, causality moves one way from low order phenomenon to higher order phenomenon

Ignores the possibility of complex higher order systems exerting a causal influence on more basic lower order systems

Biogenetic determinism moves explanation of social and behavioral problems to the genes

Individual rather than social conditions or economic inequities is responsible for problems

However, the individual is not responsible, the genes are responsible

Many modern individuals have a sense of helplessness due to a naive

reductionism (obesity and depression good examples)

Much effort is put towards finding new drugs that will solve

social/interpersonal/emotional/economic/political

problemsSlide11

Alternatives to Reductionism

Holism –

systems cannot be understood by taking them apart

Emergent Properties

– as components associate into systems, new properties of the systems emerge which cannot be predicted from the properties of the components (e.g.,

hydrogen + oxygen

water)

Complex systems science

– systems form hierarchies of increasing complexity and exhibit adaptive behavior at each level of

analysis

Homeostasis

Feedback loops

Cross-level linkagesSlide12

http://necsi.org/projects/mclemens/cs_char.gifSlide13

Problems with Complex systems

Goals of science are the same (understanding, prediction, and control) but the methods are different

Requires different frameworks and methodologies which are not as well developed as experimental reductionism

Mathematical simulations

Complex statistical modeling

Nonlinear models

Multilevel

models

Evaluation of real-world interventions

It becomes difficult to make reassuring cause and effect

statements;

Scientists are

forced

to live with

uncertainty.

It becomes difficult to create unambiguous mechanistic explanationsSlide14

Example: Obesity Epidemic

The United States and other developed countries are experiencing an epidemic of obesity

Why is this happening?

What can be done to reverse the trends?

Problem is so serious that life expectancies may begin to decline by the middle of the 21

st

centurySlide15

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1985

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14%Slide16

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1986

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14%Slide17

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1987

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14%Slide18

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1988

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14%Slide19

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1989

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14%Slide20

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1990

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14%Slide21

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1991

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% Slide22

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1992

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% Slide23

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1993

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% Slide24

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1994

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% Slide25

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1995

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% Slide26

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1996

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% Slide27

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1997

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19%

≥20%Slide28

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1998

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19%

≥20%Slide29

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 1999

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19%

≥20%Slide30

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2000

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19%

≥20%Slide31

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2001

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24%

≥25%Slide32

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2002

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24%

≥25%Slide33

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2003

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24%

≥25%Slide34

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2004

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24%

≥25%Slide35

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2005

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24% 25%–29% ≥30%

Slide36

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2006

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24% 25%–29% ≥30%

Slide37

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2007

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24% 25%–29% ≥30%

Slide38

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2008

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24% 25%–29% ≥30%

Slide39

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2009

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24% 25%–29% ≥30%

Slide40

Obesity Trends* Among U.S. Adults

BRFSS, 2010

(*BMI ≥30, or ~ 30 lbs. overweight for 5’ 4” person)

No Data <10% 10%–14% 15%–19% 20%–24% 25%–29% ≥30%

Slide41

How can we explain this?

What are some possible explanations?

Is there a single cause we need to be looking for?

If there are multiple causes, how do we study them?

Are the causes additive or synergistic?

Do the causes cascade across levels of analysis (e.g., macroeconomic factors influencing individual behaviors)?

Does our framework (reductionism versus complex systems science) make a difference in how we approach these problems?Slide42
Slide43

Reflections

The question is not which approach is the best approach, but which is the best for solving a specific problem

Reductionism does not automatically lead to translation research

Complex systems science may have much more translation potential

Complex systems science requires interdisciplinary research, different methodological approaches, and the abandonment of simple one-cause explanationsSlide44

What characterizes “translation” research?

Addresses problems in clinical care and population health

Evidence-based (based on best science available)

Involves transfer of knowledge and or methods across disciplinary boundaries

Requires consideration of context (target is imbedded in real-world

systems)

Coalitions

and partnerships

E

ngagement

of

communities

Moves away from trying to find a single causal factor and towardsSlide45
Slide46

Familiar example of complex systems approach to improve chronic disease managementSlide47

Challenges

Personalized medicine?

Matching drugs to genes

How about matching treatment to other systems that are influencing health

Family

Neighborhood

Work setting

Psychology (cognition and emotion)

Health services research?

Are there gains to be had from adopting complex systems framework?

Need viable alternatives to the clinical trial

Implementation science?

Can methods such as continuous quality improvement become scientific tools for answering questions about improving clinical care and population health

What other methods can be adapted?