/
The Heritability of Partisan AttachmentJaime E. SettleChristopher T. D The Heritability of Partisan AttachmentJaime E. SettleChristopher T. D

The Heritability of Partisan AttachmentJaime E. SettleChristopher T. D - PDF document

sherrill-nordquist
sherrill-nordquist . @sherrill-nordquist
Follow
460 views
Uploaded On 2015-08-06

The Heritability of Partisan AttachmentJaime E. SettleChristopher T. D - PPT Presentation

better conceived as instrumental changeable andresponsive to current conditions and attitudes towardcontemporary political events As opposed to thesocial psychological interpretations of partisansh ID: 101844

better conceived instrumental changeable

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Pdf The PPT/PDF document "The Heritability of Partisan AttachmentJ..." is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

The Heritability of Partisan AttachmentJaime E. SettleChristopher T. DawesJames H. FowlerUniversity of California, San DiegoOne of the strongest regularities in the empirical political science literature is the well-known correlation in parentand child partisan behavior. Until recently, this phenomenon was thought to result solely from parental socialization,but new evidence on genetic sources of behavior suggests it might also be due to heritability. In this article, the authorshypothesize that genes contribute to variation in a general tendency toward strength of partisanship. Using data col-lected at the Twins Days Festival, the authors compare the similarity of partisan strength in identical twins who shareall of their genes to the similarity of partisan strength in nonidentical twins who share only half of their genes. Theresults show that heritability accounts for almost half of the variance in strength of partisan attachment, suggestingwe should pay closer attention to the role of biology in the expression of important political behaviors.Keywords:political psychology; political methodology; public opinion and political participationThe study of partisanship occupies a vast part of thepolitical behavior literature because of the com-plexity of what it means, how it forms, and what itpredicts (e.g., Campbell et al. 1960; Fiorina 1981;Niemi and Jennings 1990; Popkin 1991; Gerber andGreen 1998). Partisanship is typically evaluated alongtwo dimensionsÑthe strength of reported partisanattachment and the direction of that attachment. Whilethere is much divergence of opinion on the nature andmeasurement of partisanship, scholars have almostexclusively focused their attention on the socializationprocess and environmental determinants of the origin,direction, and intensity of partisanship. However,recent work has demonstrated that heredityplays arole in closely related political behaviors, such aspolitical attitudes (Alford, Funk, and Hibbing 2005;Hatemi et al. 2007; Tesser 1993), political orientations(Alford, Funk, and Hibbing 2005; Settle et al. 2008),voting behavior (Fowler, Baker, and Dawes 2008;Fowler and Dawes 2008; Dawes and Fowler 2008), andtrust (Cesarini et al. 2008). The developing consensusthat genes play an important role in political behaviorleads us to believe that heritability could also help toexplain one of the remaining questions in the parti-sanship literature: what contributes to the underlyingstrength of a personÕs partisan identity?What Is Partisanship?Party identification was originally conceived as anaffective attachment resulting from the process ofsocialization (Campbell et al. 1960), stemming fromchildhood and reflecting the influences of the imme-diate social milieu and the family (Hyman 1959;Greenstein 1965). Subsequent work built on thissocial psychological view argued that identificationwith a particular party is based on images of thatparty as a social group (Gerber and Green 1998;Green, Palmquist, and Schickler 2002; Fowler andKam 2007). The authors of The American Voter(Campbell et al. 1960) essentially viewed strength ofpartisanship as a fixed factor that could be used topredict political behavior, but they could only specu-late as to why or how it was fixed.Since the 1970s, a debate in the literature has con-tested whether partisanship is affective, nearlyimmutable, and emotionally based or whether it isJaime E. Settle, PhD Candidate, University of California, San Diego; e-mail: jsettle@ucsd.edu.Christopher T. Dawes, PhD Candidate, University of California,San Diego; e-mail: cdawes@ucsd.edu.James H. Fowler, Associate Professor of Political Science,University of California, San Diego; e-mail: jhfowler@ucsd.edu.AuthorsÕNote:We would like to thank Peter Hatemi and RoseMcDermott for comments and encouragement. We would alsolike to thank Ira Carmen and Gene Robinson for putting togetherthe Biology and Politics conference at the University of Illinoisat Urbana-Champaign, where we first presented this work; theorganizers of the Twins Days Festival in Twinsburg, Ohio; andthe National Science Foundation (grant SES-0719404) andInstitute of Government Affairs at the University of California,Davis, for generous research support. better conceived as instrumental, changeable, andresponsive to current conditions and attitudes towardcontemporary political events. As opposed to thesocial psychological interpretations of partisanship,instrumental theories view partisan attachment as an 1 2 ) doesnot seem to play a significant role (0 percent, CI =0percent, 37 percent). Measures of model fit indicatethat an AE model is better than the ACE model. All ofthe models presented include controls for age andgender. These control variables only influence theestimation of the three thresholds and do not enterinto the estimation of a for heritability in determining partisan strength intro- strength could be viewed as the unshared environ-mental factors that combine with genetic predisposi-tions to change partisanship strength in an individual.This would be consistent with Scarr andMcCartneyÕs (1983) theory of how genetic and envi-ronmental differences combine to produce variationin development. They argued that the role of geno-type determines which environments are experiencedby individuals and which environments individualsseek for themselves; essentially, people seek outexperiences to reinforce their genetic predispositions.Those inclined to be partisan seek out opportunitiesto do so, which has the effect of strengthening theirpartisan attachment even further. This finding helpsclarify some of the debate over the endogenous ver-sus exogenous nature of partisan strength by demon-strating roles for both. People may have a geneticpredisposition toward developing strong attachmentto a political party, but there is still room for this pre-disposition to be shaped by both shared and uniqueenvironments.Another implication of our results is that we mightbetter think of the acquisition of partisanship in twodistinct parts, one strongly influenced by genes andthe other strongly influenced by the environment. Theliterature has already conceptualized party identifica-tion as consisting of two components: a directioncomponent, which indicates the specific party withwhich an individual identifies; and a strength compo-nent, which reflects the intensity of that identification(Converse 1976). Our results suggest that partisanintensity is heritable but partisan direction is not.Our finding is consistent with the pattern of findingsfrom studies of other social behaviors, such as reli-gious beliefs and practices. Strength of attachment to agroup, such as strength of partisanship or religiosity,has a strong heritable component, perhaps because ofits relationship to fundamental processes in earlyhuman history. For example, we can imagine that thestrength of oneÕs affiliation and association withgroups was of more consequence when survivaldepended more directly on group cooperation.Evolutionary models of cooperation show that someenvironments favor group participation in the produc-tion of public goods, while other environments favorself-reliance because of the competition between con-tributors and free-riders (Fowler 2005; Hauert et al.2007). These models predict heterogeneity in strate-gies, with some individuals joining groups and otherstrying to survive on their own. It is possible that this of attachment once it has been formed. Second, ourmeasure of partisan attachment, while frequentlyemployed in the literature, has been subject to the cri-tique that it does not adequately address the theoreti-cal differences between partisans and nonpartisans;partisanship appears to be multidimensional and non-monotonic (Kamieniecki 1988; Petrocik 1974), andconsequently, standard scales of partisanship strengthmay not adequately address how partisanship is cor-related with political behavior. The 7-point scale usedin our study may be best for measuring attitudestoward political parties in general because the maindifferences between subgroups of independent votersis in their orientation toward the symbols of politicalindependence (Craig 1985). However, a scale such asthat discussed in Greene (2002) or Weisberg andHasecke (1999) may better measure the social-psychological identity aspect of partisan strength.Third, the ACE model used in this study estab-lishes that genes do play a role in partisan attachment,but it cannot expose the exact mechanism by whichgenes and the environment interact to produce thephenotype. To best understand partisan attachment,in the future, we must also examine these interactioneffects. The significant contribution of the unsharedenvironment in our study opens the door for an exam-ination of factors that could serve as mediators for thegene-attachment mechanism. It seems likely that weare tapping into a general predisposition towardattachment, interest, or engagement and that environ-mental factors can play an important role in channel-ing that predisposition into behavior.Although our use of the ACE model does not allowus to specify the contribution of any one gene in par-ticular, the most likely candidate of genes identifiedto date is the DRD2 gene (Dawes and Fowler 2008).The A1 allele of this gene has been related todecreased dopamine signaling in the brain (Jonssonet al. 1999), and the consequences of altereddopamine receptors include social detachment (Breieret al. 1998; Farde, Gustavsson, and Jonsson 1997;Jonsson et al. 1999), social alienation (Hill et al.1999), antisocial personality disorder (Ponce et al. 2003),and avoidant personality types (Blum et al. 1997). Allfour of these behaviors could reasonably be linkedto a decreased tendency toward group attachment andaffiliation, and studies testing for various alleles ofthis gene have shown an association between DRD2,partisan attachment, and voting (Dawes and Fowler2008).The resurgence of interest in biopolitics calls for asystematic approach to the study of political heritabilityto move toward a more explanatory basis for theresearch program (Fowler and Schreiber 2008), andscholars have made significant strides in this direction(Alford and Hibbing 2008; Medland and Hatemi forth-coming). The first step, like the one we take here, is toestablish which political behaviors are heritable andwhich are not. Scientists in other fields continue toexplore the mechanisms by which genetic variationaffects general psychological and behavioral tenden-cies. The next step for political scientists is to applythis knowledge in the search for specific genes, neuraland physiological mechanisms that may underlie polit-ical behaviors, as well as their potential evolutionarybasis (McDermott, Fowler, and Smirnov 2008). In par-ticular, it is extremely important to combine our newunderstanding of biology with our prior investigationsof environmental causes of political behavior.For example, Fowler, Baker, and Dawes (2008)originally showed that political participation is heri-table, and this study was followed by a molecular a2/(a2+c2+e2). We use the software package MX to estimatethis structural equations model (Neale et al. 2006). Since ourdependent variable is (ordered) categorical, the modelassumes that latent distribution of partisan strength is normaleven though we only observe four distinct values. Therefore,in addition to estimating a2, c2 independent, butcloser to Republicans; Republican; or strong Republican. The the minimum value and very conservativeand very liberalas themaximum values.3. The ACE model (additive genetic factors [A], shared orcommon environmental factors [C], and unshared environmentalfactors [E]) assumes that genetic effects are only additive; there-fore there are no dominance effects. However, a dizygotic (DZ)correlation that is less than half of the monozygotic (MZ) corre-lation is generally considered evidence of dominance. While thisis the case for our point estimates, the confidence interval for theDZ correlations is fairly wide due to our small sample size. Toformally test the null hypothesis that the DZ correlation is at leastas large as half the MZ correlation, we bootstrapped one thou-sand MZ and DZ correlation coefficients. We failed to reject thenull hypothesis that DZ $0.5 %MZ (p =.71), suggesting that theACE model is the correct one to use for estimation.ReferencesAbramson, Paul. 1976. Generational change and the decline ofparty identification in America: 1952-1974. AmericanPolitical Science Review70 (June): 469-78.Abramson, Paul. 1979a. Comment: On the relationshipbetween age and party identification. Political Methodology effects. American Journal of Political Science 23(February): 78-96.Abramson, P. R., and C. W. Ostrom. 1991. Macropartisanship:An empirical reassessment. American Political ScienceReview85:181-92.Abramson, P. R., and C. W. Ostrom. 1992. Response. AmericanPolitical Science Review86:481-86.Abramson, P. R., and C. W. Ostrom. 1994a. Question wordingand partisanship. Public Opinion Quarterly58:21-48.Abramson, P. R., and C. W. Ostrom. 1994b. Response. AmericanPolitical Science Review88:955-58.Achen, Christopher. 1992. Social psychology, demographic vari-ables, and linear regression: Breaking the iron triangle in vot-ing research. Political Behavior14 (3): 195Ð211.Alford, John, Carolyn Funk, and John Hibbing. 2005. Are politi-cal orientations genetically transmitted? American PoliticalScience Review99:153-67.Alford, John, and John Hibbing. 2008. The new empirical biopol-itics. Annual Review of Political Science11:183-203.Ashenfelter, Orley, and Alan Krueger. 1994. Estimates of the eco-nomic return to schooling from a new sample of twins.American Economic Review tal influences on human psychological differences. Journal ofNeurobiology54 (1): 4-45.Bouchard, T. J., M. McGue, D. Lykken, and A. Tellegen. 1999.Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: Genetics and environmen-tal influences and personality correlates. Twin Research2:88-89.Bouchard, T. J., N. Segal, A. Tellegen, M. McGue, M. Keys, andR. Krueger. 2003. Evidence for the construct validity and her-itabilty of the Wilson Patterson Conservatism Scale: A rearedapart twins study of social attitudes. Journal of Personalityand Individual Differences34:959-69.Bouchard, T. J., N. Segal, A. Tellegen, M. McGue, M. Keys, andR. Krueger. 2004. Genetic influence on social attitudes:Another challenge to psychology from behavior genetics. InBehavior genetic principles: Development, personality andpsychopathology, ed. L. DiLalla, pp. 89-104. Washington,DC: American Psychological Association Press.Breier, A., L. Kestler, C. Adler, I. Elman, N. Wiesenfeld, A.Malhotra, and D. Pickar. 1998. Dopamine D2 receptor densityand personal detachment in healthy subjects. AmericanJournal of Psychiatry155:1440Ð42.Brody, R. A., D. W. Brady, and V. Heitshusen. 1994. Accounting . Chicago:University of Chicago Press.Campbell, A. G. Gurin, and W. E. Miller. 1954. The voterdecides. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.Cesarini, David, Christopher T. Dawes, James H. Fowler, MagnusJohannesson, Paul Lichtenstein, and Bjšrn Wallace. 2008.Heritability of cooperative behavior in the trust game.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 73 (2): 472-88.Mackuen, Michael B., Robert S. Erikson, and James Stimson.1989. Macropartisanship. American Political Science Review42:661-89.Martin, N. G., L. J. Eaves, A. C. Heath, Rosemary Jardine, LynnFeingold, and H. J. Eysenck. 1986. Transmission of socialattitudes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science83(12): 4364-68.Mattei, Franco, and Richard G. Niemi. 1991. Unrealized parti-sans, realized independents, and the intergenerational trans-mission of partisan identification. Journal of Politics53 (1):161-74.McDermott, Rose, James H. Fowler, and Oleg Smirnov. 2008. Onthe evolutionary origin of prospect theory preferences.Journal of Politics70 (2): 335-50.Medland, Sarah E. and Peter K. Hatemi. forthcoming. PoliticalScience, Behavior Genetics and Twin Studies: a MethodologicalPrimer.Political Analysis. Miller, Warren E., and J. Merrill Shanks. 1996. The newAmerican voter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Neale, M. C., S. M. Boker, G. Xie, and H. H. Maes. 2006. MX:Statistical modeling. 7th ed. Richmond: Virginia CommonwealthUniversity Department of Psychiatry.Neale, M. C., and L. R. Cardon. 1992. Methodology for genetic stud-ies of twins and families. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.