/
Response Biases Response Biases

Response Biases - PowerPoint Presentation

pamella-moone
pamella-moone . @pamella-moone
Follow
390 views
Uploaded On 2015-10-14

Response Biases - PPT Presentation

in Survey Research Hans Baumgartner Smeal Professor of Marketing Smeal College of Business Penn State University Response biases when a researcher conducts a survey the expectation is that the information collected will be a faithful representation of reality ID: 160068

item items reversed response items item response reversed satisficing reversal negation respondents survey ers responding negated bias model method

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Presentation The PPT/PDF document "Response Biases" is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

Slide1

Response Biases in Survey Research

Hans Baumgartner

Smeal Professor of Marketing

Smeal College of Business, Penn State UniversitySlide2

Response biaseswhen a researcher conducts a survey, the expectation is that the information collected will be a faithful representation of reality;unfortunately, this is often not the case, and survey researchers have identified many different sources of error in surveys;these errors may contaminate the research results and limit the managerial usefulness of the findings;if the response provided by a respondent does not fully reflect the “true” response, a response (or measurement) error occurs (random or systematic);

response biases (systematic response errors) can happen at any of the four stages of the response process, are elicited by different aspects of the survey, and are due to a variety of psychological mechanisms;Slide3

T

E

M

T

1

T

2

E

2

E

1

M

1

M

2

The relationship between observed measurements and constructs of interest

The total variability of observed scores consists of trait (substantive), random error, and systematic error (method) variance.

Random and systematic errors are likely to confound relation-ships between measures and constructs and between different constructs.

They also complicate

the comparison of means.Slide4

Outline of the talkMisresponse to reversed and negated itemsItem reversal and negation, types of misresponse, and mechanismsReversed item bias: An integrative modelEye tracking of survey responding to reversed and negated itemsItem grouping and discriminant validityExtreme and midpoint responding as satisficing strategies in online surveys

Stylistic response tendencies over the course of a surveySlide5

The issue of item reversalShould reverse-keyed items (also called oppositely-keyed, reversed-polarity, reverse-worded, negatively worded, negatively-keyed, keyed-false, or simply reversed items) be included in multi-item summative scales?If reversed items are to be used, does it matter whether the reversal is achieved through negation or through other means?What’s the link between reversal and negation, what types of MR result, what psychological mechanisms are involved, and how can MR be avoided?Slide6

Item reversal vs. item negationauthors often fail to draw a clear distinction between reversals and negations and use ambiguous terms such as ‘negatively worded items’, which makes it unclear whether they refer to reversed or negated items, or both;examples from the Material Values scale (Richins and Dawson 1992):

It sometimes bothers me quite a bit that I can't afford to buy all the things I’d like.

I have all the things I really need to enjoy life.

I wouldn't be any happier if I owned nicer things.Slide7

Item negationitems can be stated either as an assertion (affirmation) or as a denial (disaffirmation) of something (Horn 1989);negation is a grammatical issue;classification of negations in terms of two dimensions: what part of speech is negated (how a word is used in a sentence: as a verb, noun/pronoun, adjective, adverb or preposition/conjunction);

how the negation is achieved (by means of particle negation, the addition of no, the use of negative affixes, negative adjectives and adverbs, negative pronouns, or negative prepositions

);Slide8

Negated by means of

Part of speech

Not,

n’t

No

negative affixes

negative adjectives

and adverbs

negative pronouns

negative prepositions

Total

Verb

[This salesperson does not make false claims.]

n.a

.

dislike, disagree, etc.

[I dislike food shopping very much.]

reluctant, hesitant, never, rarely, seldom, hardly (ever), less, little, etc.

[I seldom daydream.]

n.a.

without, instead of, rather than, etc.

[This supplier sometimes promises to do things without actually doing them later.]

135 (44.7%)

5 (1.7%)

26 (8.6%)

5 (1.7%)

171 (56.6%)

Noun/

pronoun

not everyone, not (only), etc.

[I and my family will consume only certain brands, not others.]

no object, no reason, no purpose, etc.

[Clipping, organizing, and using coupons is no fun.]

discomfort, disagreement, etc.

[There is considerable disagreement as to the future directions that this hospital should take.]

little, few, a lack of, none of the, not much, neither of, etc.

[Many times I feel that I have little influence over things that happen to me.]

no-one, nobody, none, nothing, etc.

[Energy is really not my problem because there is simply nothing I can do about it.]

except for, without, with the exception of, instead of, rather than, etc.

[American people should always buy American-made products instead of imports.]

4 (1.3%)

17 (5.6%)

5 (1.7%)

10 (3.3%)

4 (1.3%)

14 (4.6%)

54 (17.9%)

Adjective

n.a

.

n.a.

uninterested, dishonest, etc.

[Most charitable organizations are dishonest.]

rarely, less, etc.

[I would be less loyal to this rep firm, if my salesperson moved to a new firm.]

n.a.

n.a

.

55 (18.2%)

4 (1.3%)

59 (19.5%)Slide9

Negated by means of

Part of speech

Not, n’t

No

negative affixes

negative adjectives

and adverbs

negative pronouns

negative prepositions

Total

Adverb

not much, etc.

[After I meet someone for the first time, I can usually remember what they look like, but not much about them.

no longer, etc.

[Hard work is no longer essential for the well-being of society.]

[I often dress unconventionally even when it's likely to offend others.]

rarely, less, etc.

[I feel I have to do things hastily and maybe less carefully in order to get everything done.]

n.a

.

n.a

.

2 (.7%)

1 (.3%)

3 (1.0%)

1 (.3%)

7 (2.3%)

Preposition/ Conjunction

not for, not (just) in, not (only) as, not until, not if (incl. unless), not because, etc.

[I enjoyed this shopping trip for its own sake, not just for the items I may have purchased.]

n.a.

n.a.

n.a.

for nothing, etc.

[I don't believe in giving anything away for nothing.]

n.a

.

10 (3.4%)

1 (.3%)

11 (3.7%)

Total

151 (50.0%)

18 (6.0%)

68 (22.5%)

41 (13.5%)

5 (1.7%)

19 (6.3%)

302Slide10

Item reversalan item is reversed if its meaning is opposite to a relevant standard of comparison (semantic issue);three senses of reversal:reversal relative to the polarity of the construct being measured;reversed relative to other items measuring the same construct:reversal relative to the first itemreversal relative to the majority of the itemsreversal relative to a respondent’s true position on the issue under consideration (Swain et al. 2008); Slide11

Integrating item negation and item reversal

Item reversal

Non-reversed

Reversed

Item negation

Non-negated

Regular (RG)

items

Talkative, enjoying talking to people

Polar opposite

(

PO)

items

Quiet, preferring to do things by oneself

Negated

Negated polar opposite (

nPO

)

items

Not quiet, preferring not to be by

oneself

Negated

regular

(

nRG

)

items

Not talkative, not getting much pleasure chatting with peopleSlide12

Misresponse to negated and reversed itemsItem

Consistent responding

Misresponse to negated items (NMR)

Misresponse to reversed items (RMR)

Misresponse

to polar opposites (POMR)

Talkative (RG)

A

A

A

A

Not talkative (

nRG

)

D

A

A

D

Quiet (PO)

D

D

A

A

Not quiet (

nPO

)

A

D

A

D

MR

→ within-participant inconsistency in response to multiple items intended to measure the same construct;Slide13

Using reversed and negated items in surveys: Some recommendationsalthough responding to reversed items is error prone, wording all questions in one direction does not solve the problem;negations should be employed sparingly, esp. if they do not result in an item reversal (note: negations come in many guises);polar opposite reversals can be beneficial (esp. at the retrieval stage), but they have to be used with care;Slide14

An integrative model of reversed item bias:Weijters, Baumgartner, and

Schillewaert (2012)

two

important method effects:

response

inconsistency between regular and reversed

items;

difference

in mean response depending on whether the first item measuring the focal construct is a regular or reversed

item;

three sources of reversed item method

bias:acquiescencecareless respondingconfirmation biasSlide15

The survey response process

(Tourangeau et al. 2000)

Attending to and interpreting survey questions

(careless responding)

Comprehension

Retrieval

Judgment

Response

Generating a retrieval strategy and retrieving relevant beliefs from memory

(confirmation bias)

Integrating the information into a judgment

Mapping the

judgment onto the response scale and answering the question

(acquiescence)Slide16

Empirical studies(net) acquiescence and carelessness explicitly measured;confirmation bias modeled via a manipulation of two item orders in the questionnaire, depending on the keying direction of the first item measuring the target construct;three item arrangements:

grouped-alternated condition (related items are grouped together and regular and reverse-keyed items are alternated);

grouped-massed condition (items

are

grouped

together, but the reverse-keyed items follow a block of regular

items, or

vice

versa;

dispersed condition (items are spread throughout the questionnaire, with unrelated buffer items spaced between the target

items); Slide17
Slide18

Results for Study 2both NARS (gNARS = .33, p < .001) and IMC (gIMC = .31, p < .001) were highly significant determinants of inconsistency bias;

the effect of NARS on inconsistency bias was invariant across item arrangement conditions, as expected;the

effect of IMC did not differ by item arrangement

condition;

the

manipulation of whether or not the first target item was reversed (FIR)

did not affect responses (although in the first study the effect was significantly negative);

the effect of FIR did not differ by item arrangement condition;Slide19

Eye tracking of survey responding(with Weijters and Pieters)eye-tracking data may provide more detailed insights into how respondents process survey questions and arrive at an answer;eye movements can be recorded unobtrusively, and eye fixations show what respondents attend to while completing a survey; Slide20

Eye tracking study101 respondents completed a Qualtrics survey and their eye movements were tracked; effective sample size is N=90;Design:each participant completed 16 four-item scales shown in a random sequence;

the fourth (target) item on each screen was an RG, nRG, PO, or nPO

item (4 scales each);Slide21

Areas of interest

AOI1a to AOI1e

AOI2a

AOI2b

AOI2c

AOI2d

AOI3a

AOI3b

AOI3c

AOI3d

AOI4a

AOI5a

AOI5b

AOI4bSlide22

  

Mean fixation duration

Percentage of

total

fixation

duration

aoi1

1.05

0.050

aoi1a

0.14

0.006 aoi1b

0.24

0.011

aoi1c

0.38 0.016

aoi1d

0.21

0.010

aoi1e

0.08 0.004

Aoi2

15.45

0.624

aoi2a

3.75

0.153

aoi2b

3.39

0.140

aoi2c

4.11 0.162 aoi2d 4.20 0.169 aoi5a 0.610.025 aoi5b (for nRG and nPO) 0.390.016aoi3 4.88 0.212 aoi3a 1.40 0.061 aoi3b 1.12 0.048 aoi3c 1.15 0.049 aoi3d 1.20

0.053aoi4

0.43

0.020other areas

2.20 0.099

Total 24.01

1.000

Fixation durations for various AOI’sSlide23

Determinants of total fixation durationfor fourth item (logaoi23dplus1)Solutions for Fixed Effects

Effect

Estimate

S. E.

t Value

Pr

> |t|

Intercept

0.6203

0.1601

3.87

0.0015

Survey

completion time

0.0013

0.0002

8.66

<.0001

Screen

sequence

-

0.0052

0.0020

-

2.650.0082 Number of words

0.0387

0.0112

3.46

0.0006

Reversal of item

0.0411

0.0185

2.23

0.0261

Negation

of item

0.1249

0.0202

6.20

<.0001

Use

of a PO in the item0.06050.01883.220.0013Note: These results are based on a mixed model with respondent and construct as random effects.Slide24

Determinants of misresponseSolutions for Fixed Effects

Effect

Estimate

S. E.

t Value

Pr

> |t|

Intercept

0.6418

0.0850

7.55

<.0001

Fixation duration for item (logaoi23plus1)

0.1394

0.0417

3.34

0..0009

Reversal of item

0.0383

0.0184

2.08

0.0380

Negation

of item

0.0159

0.0185

0.86

0.3912

Use

of a PO in the item

0.1197

0.0187

6.40

<.0001

Note: These results are based on a mixed model with

dist

=gamma and construct as a random effect.Slide25

Item grouping and discriminant validity (Weijters, de Beuckelaer, and Baumgartner, forthcoming)question whether items belonging to the same scale should be grouped or randomized:grouped format is less cognitively demanding and often improves convergent validity;random format may reduce demand effects, respondent satisfacing, and carryover effects, as well as faking;

effect of item grouping on discriminant validity:grouping of items enhances discriminant validity (Harrison and McLaughlin 1996);

item grouping may lead to discriminant validity even when there should be none;Slide26

Method523 respondents from an online U.S. panelquestionnaire contained the 8-item frugality scale of Lastovicka et al. (1999) and 32 filler items;frugality scale presented in two random blocks of 4 items each, with the 32 filler items in betweenCondition 1: 1-2-3-4 vs. 5-6-7-8Condition 2: 1-2-7-8 vs. 3-4-5-6within blocks item order was randomized across respondents;Slide27

Estimated modelsSlide28

ResultsCOND

Model

MODEL

²

DF

CFI

TLI

SRMR

RMSEA

Cond 1

Two factors

Hypothesized model

53.82

19

.964

.946

.037

.083

Best of alternative permutations

198.13

19

.810

.720

.090

.188

Average of alternative permutations

221.84

19

.788

.687

.096

.200

One factor

One-factor

model

234.04

20

.776

.686

.095

.200Cond 2Two factors Hypothesized model43.6719.961.943.040.071Best of alternative permutations88.2419.880.823.069.120Average of alternative permutations129.9319.825.743.076.151

 One factor

One-factor model140.83

20.810

.734.076.154Slide29

Results (cont’d)Slide30

Results (cont’d)

Condition 1

Condition 2

Factor 1

Average Variance Extracted (AVE)

.54

.53

Composite Reliability (CR

)

.82

.81

Factor 2

Average Variance Extracted (AVE)

.61

.40

Composite Reliability (CR)

.86

.71

Factors 1 and 2

Correlation

.61

.61

Shared Variance (SV)

.38

.38Slide31

Extreme and midpoint responding as satisficing strategies in online surveys(Weijters and Baumgartner)when respondents minimize the amount of effort they invest in formulating responses to questionnaire items by selecting the first response that is deemed good enough, they are said to be satisficing; when respondents put in the cognitive resources required to arrive at an optimal response, they are optimizing (Krosnick

1991); the effectiveness of procedural

remedies to prevent or at least reduce satisficing

(

MacKenzie

&

Podsakoff

2012

) is limited;post hoc indices designed to identify

satisficers often exhibit limited convergent validity and unambiguous cutoff values are often unavailable; Slide32

Satisficing in online surveys (cont’d)online surveys are likely to contain data from respondents who are satisficing, but what will be the consequences?we review satisficing and related measures that have been proposed in the

literature and propose a new measure called OPTIM;we investigate the

effect

of satisficing

on two

stylistic response

tendencies (ERS and MRS)

and we demonstrate that the direction of the relationship varies across

individuals

;Slide33

The concept of satisficingthe notion of satisficing is consistent with the view of people as cognitive misers (Fiske and Taylor 1991);satisficing is conceptually similar to carelessness, inattentiveness, insufficient effort responding, and content-nonresponsive, content-independent, noncontingent

, inconsistent, variable or random responding;

Krosnick

(1991) argues that in weak forms of satisficing each of the four steps

of the response process (comprehension, retrieval, judgment, response) might

be compromised to some extent, whereas in strong forms of satisficing the second and third steps might be skipped

entirely

;Slide34

Measures of satisficing 

Dedicated measures

 

Special items or scales are included in the questionnaire to measure satisficing

No

dedicated

measures

 

Satisficing is inferred from respondents’ answers to

substantive questions

Direct measurement

 

Satisficing is assessed directly by measuring respondents’ tendency to minimize time and effort when responding to a survey

 

Category

1

 Self-reported effort (e.g., I carefully read every survey item).

 

Category 2

 Response time

Indirect measurement

Satisficing is assessed indirectly based on the presumed consequences of respondents’ attempts to minimize time and effort on the quality of

responses

 Category 3 Quality of responses to special items or scales (e.g. bogus items, instructed response items)

 

 

Category

4

 

Quality of responses to substantive questions (e.g., outlier analysis, lack of consistency of responses, excessive consistency of responses

)Slide35

Measures of satisficing (cont’d)a single-category measure is unlikely to assess satisficing adequately;direct measures of satisficing are desirable (esp. response time measures);bogus items and IMC’s have limitations;response differentiation for unrelated items might be a good outcome-based measure;Slide36

A new measure of satisficingoptimizing as the time-intensive differentiation of responses to items that are homogeneous in form but heterogeneous in content:OPTIM=log(TIME*DIFF)survey duration:

input side of effort (indicator of the cognitive resources invested by a respondent);time taken to complete the survey (in minutes), rescaled to a range of 0 to 10;

response differentiation:

output side of effort (indicator of optimizing for heterogeneous items);

DIFF = (f1+1)*(f2+1)*(f3+1)*(f4+1)*(f5+1

), rescaled to a range of 0 to 10;Slide37

ERS and MRS as satisficing strategiesprevious research suggests that both ERS and MRS may be used as satisficing strategies (even though ERS and MRS tend to be negatively correlated), although the empirical findings have not been very consistent;different respondents may use different satisficing strategies: some respondents may simplify the rating task by only using the extreme scale

positions (resulting in increased ERS);

others

may refrain from thinking things through and taking sides (resulting in increased MRS

); Slide38

Methodtwo online studies with Belgian (n=320) and Dutch (n=401) respondents;in dataset A 10 heterogeneous attitudinal items and in dataset B Greenleaf’s (1992) ERS scale;these items were used to construct the ERS (number of extreme responses), MRS (number of midpoint responses) and DIFF measures; survey duration was measured unobtrusively; use of a multivariate Poisson regression mixture model of ERS and MRS on

OPTIM; Slide39

ModelSlide40

Regression estimates by class 

 

DV

Intercept

B

SE

90% CI

DATASET A

Class 1 (46.3%)

ERS

2.64

-.54

.10

[-.70, -.38]

Extreme responders

MRS

-3.08

1.39

.20

[1.06, 1.72]

Class 2 (47.1%)

ERS

-1.04

.90

.11[.72, 1.08]

Yeah-sayers

MRS

-.53

.53

.11

[.35, .71]

Class 3

(

6.7%)

ERS

-7.25

3.04

.83

[1.67, 4.41]

 

Midpoint responders

MRS2.39-.49.14[-.72, -.26]DATASET B Class 1 (43.7%)ERS-1.731.13.12[.93, 1.33]Midpoint respondersMRS2.59-.53.08[-.66, -.40]Class 2 (35.7%)ERS-.41.88.39[.24, 1.52]Yeah-sayersMRS

.24.44.25

[.03, .85]Class 3 (20.6%)

ERS2.91

-.48.25[-.89, -.07]

 Extreme responders

MRS-.40.57

.15

[.32, .82]Slide41
Slide42

Dataset A

Dataset BSlide43

DiscussionOPTIM as an unobtrusive measure that integrates several aspects of optimizing/satisficing;across two distinct samples, three satisficing segments emerged:extreme respondersmidpoint respondersacquiescent respondersOPTIM is useful if a continuous measure of satisficing is required, but it may be less useful as a screening device for careless responders; Slide44

Stylistic response tendencies over the course of a survey (Baumgartner and Weijters)three perspectives on stylistic responding:nonexistence of response styles (complete lack of consistency); instability of response styles (local consistency);stability of response styles (global consistency); Weijters et al. (2010) showed that

the nonexistence of response styles was strongly contradicted by the empirical evidence for both extreme responding and acquiescent responding;

there was a strong stable component in the ratings; and

there as a weaker local component (as indicated by a small time-invariant autoregressive effect);Slide45

Unresolved questionshow do stylistic response tendencies evolve over the course of a questionnaire?prior research has only considered the effect of stylistic responding on the covariance structure of items or sets of items and has ignored the mean structure;are there individual

differences in both the extent to which stylistic response tendencies occur across respondents and the manner in which stylistic response tendencies evolve over the course of a survey?

prior research has not

emphasized heterogeneity in stylistic response tendencies across

people; Slide46

ALT modelSlide47

Integrated ALT model for NARS and ERSSlide48

Methoddata from 523 online respondents;each participant responded to a random selection of eight out of 16 possible four-item scales shown on eight consecutive screens in random order;eight separate response style indices were computed for both (net) acquiescence response style or NARS (i.e., respondents’ tendency to express more agreement than disagreement) and extreme response style or ERS (i.e., respondents’ disproportionate use of more extreme response options

);the design

guarantees that there is no systematic similarity in substantive content over the sequence of eight scales across

respondents; Slide49

Results

Estimate

SE

T

p

95% confidence interval

Means

iERS

.918

.019

49.03

< .001

[ .881, .955]

sERS

-.009

.003

-2.77

.006

[ -.015, -.003]

iNARS

3.298

.027

123.59

< .001

[3.246, 3.350]

sNARS

-.019

.005

-3.77

< .001

[ -.029, -.009]

Variances

iERS

.105

.012

9.00

< .001

[ .082, .128]

sERS

.001

.000

2.49.013[ .000, .002]iNARS.155.0246.38< .001[ .108, .203]sNARS.001.0011.30.195[-.001, .003]CorrelationsiERS with sERS -.312.120-2.60.009[-.547, -.077]iERS with iNARS.491

.0825.98< .001

[ .330, .651]iERS

with sNARS-.189.204

-.93.352

[-.588, .210]iNARS with

sNARS-.583.132

-4.42

< .001

[-.842, -.325]

sERS

with

iNARS

-.074

.172

-.43

.669

[-.411, .264]

sERS

with

sNARS

.129

.388

.33

.740

[-.632, .889]Slide50

NARS and ERS trajectoriesSlide51

Distribution of the slope factor for ERSSlide52

Response distributions on the first and the last screen of the questionnaireSlide53

Backup slidesSlide54

A comprehensive model of measurement error

y

ijt

=

ijt

+ 

ijt

jt

+ 

ijt

+ 

ijt

y

ijt

a person’s observed score on the i

th

measure of construct j at time t

jt  a person’s

unobserved score for construct j at time tijt  systematic error score

ijt  random error score

ijt  coefficient (factor loading) relating yijt to jtijt 

intercept term (additive bias)

systematic

error

random

errorSlide55

Empirical datawe analyzed items from volumes 1 through 36 of JCR (1974 till the end of 2009) and volumes 1 through 46 of JMR (1964 to 2009);we included all Likert-type scales for which the items making up the scale were reproduced in the article and factor loadings or item-total correlations were reported;total of 66 articles in which information about 1330 items measuring 314 factors was provided;of the 1330 distinct items in the data set, 608 came from JCR and 722 from JMR;Slide56

Item reversal (cont’d)in our data set of 1330 items, between 83 and 86 percent of items were nonreversed (depending on the definition of reversal);the proportion of factors (or subfactors in the case of multi-factor constructs) that do not contain reversed items was 70 percent;only 8 percent of factors (out of 314 factors) were composed of an equal number of reversed and

nonreversed items (i.e., the scale was balanced); Slide57

Cross-classification of negation and reversal

Reversal relative to

Polarity of construct

Polarity of first item

Dominant keying direction

Total

Non-reversed

Reversed

Non-reversed

Reversed

Non-reversed

Balanced

Reversed

No

negation(s)

71.1%

8.7%

71.0%

8.7%

70.3%

5.2%

4.2%

79.7%

Negation(s

)

11.7%

8.7%

15.4%

4.9%

13.7%

2.9%

3.7%

20.3%

Total

82.8%

17.4%

86.4%

13.6%

84.0%

8.1%

7.9%

100.0%Slide58

Theoretical explanations of MR:

Reversal ambiguity and comprehension

Rs

may not view antonyms as polar opposites [POMR];

contradictories vs. contraries:

Antonym reversals can be contradictories or contraries, depending on whether they are bounded or unbounded (

Paradis

and

Willners

2006);

Negation reversals are contradictories if the core concept is the same; the situation is more complicated for the negation of bounded and unbounded antonyms;

simultaneous disagreement with contraries is more likely when items are worded extremely (McPherson and Mohr 2005);

“Buddhism’s ontology and epistemology appear to make East Asians relatively comfortable with apparent contradictions” (Wong et al. 2003, p. 86) [RMR];Slide59

Theoretical explanations of MR:ComprehensionCareless responding (Schmitt and Stults 1985):respondents fail to pay careful attention to individual items and respond based on their overall position on an issue [RMR];more likely when a reversed item is preceded by a block of nonreversed items;Remedies:

make Rs more attentive and/or explicitly alert them to the presence of reversed items;use balanced scales, alternate the keying direction, and disperse the items;Slide60

Theoretical explanations of MR:Comprehension (cont’d)Reversal ambiguity:Rs may not view antonyms as polar opposites [POMR];“Buddhism’s ontology and epistemology appear to make East Asians relatively comfortable with apparent contradictions” (Wong et al. 2003, p. 86) [RMR];contradictories vs. contraries:

Negated statements are contradictories;

Antonyms can be contradictories or contraries, depending on whether they are bounded or unbounded (

Paradis

and

Willners

2006);

simultaneous disagreement is more likely when items are worded extremely (McPherson and Mohr 2005);Slide61

Theoretical explanations of MR:Comprehension (cont’d)Remedies:use more sophisticated procedures to identify appropriate antonyms (formulate linguistic contrasts in two stages; see Dickson and Albaum 1977);may be particularly useful in cross-cultural research;bounded antonyms have to be pretested and unbounded antonyms have to be used with care;extreme statements should be avoided;Slide62

Theoretical explanations of MR:RetrievalItem-wording effects:Confirmation bias (Davies 2003; Kunda et al. 1993);Directly applicable to antonymic reversals;For negation reversals, confirmation bias can lead to MR if a non-negated polar opposite schema is readily available (Mayo et al. 2004); Remedies:Use polar opposite reversals to get richer belief samples, even though they may increase apparent MR;

Negation reversals have few retrieval benefits;Slide63

Theoretical explanations of MR:RetrievalPositioning effects:Dispersed PO items reduce carryover effects and can increase coverage, but the task is more taxing for Rs and internal consistency may suffer;Item similarity may determine whether Rs engage in additional retrieval when items are grouped together;Remedies: The use of dispersed antonyms should encourage the generation of distinct belief samples;

Avoid very similar (negated) statements when items are grouped;Slide64

Theoretical explanations of MR:JudgmentItem verification difficulty (Carpenter and Just 1975; Swain et al. 2008):MR is a function of the complexity of verifying the truth or falsity of an item relative to one’s true beliefs, which depends on whether the item is stated as an affirmation or negation [NMR];Remedies:Negations are problematic because they increase the likelihood of making mistakes (remember there are many types of negations);Negated polar opposites are most error-prone;Mix of regular and PO reversals should be best;Slide65

Item verification difficulty

Truth value

True

False

Negation

Affirmation

MRSlide66

Theoretical explanations of MR:ResponseAcquiescence: Rs initially accept a statement and subsequently re-consider it based on extant evidence; the first stage is automatic, the second requires effort (Knowles and Condon 1999) [RMR];Remedies: Although response styles are largely individual difference variables, situational factors may be under the control of the researcher (e.g., reduce the cognitive load for Rs);Problems with online surveys;Slide67

Theoretical explanations of MR:Response (cont’d)Asymmetric scale interpretation: the midpoint of the rating scale may not be the boundary between agreement and disagreement for Rs (esp. if the response categories are not labeled; cf. Gannon and Ostrom 1996) [RMR];Remedies:Use fully labeled 5- or 7-point response scales;Slide68

Careless responding

respondents do not always pay attention to the instructions, the wording of the question, or the response options before answering survey questions because of satisficing;

respondents may form expectations about what is being measured and respond to individual items based on their overall position concerning the focal issue, rather than specific item content;

this can result in inconsistent responding to reverse-keyed items;

esp. likely when constructs are labeled and when grouped-massed item positioning is used;

measurement:

instructional manipulation checks (IMC), bogus items, and self-report measures of response quality

response times

post hoc response consistency indices (too much or too little)Slide69

Examples

of IMC’s

Between 14% and 46% of respondents failed this test in

Oppenheimer et al. (2009)Slide70

Examples of IMC’s

About 7% of

respondents (out of over 1000) failed this test (see

Oppenheimer et al. 2009)Slide71

(Dis)Acquiescence

tendency to agree (ARS) or disagree (DARS) with items regardless of content (agreement or

yea-saying vs. disagreement or nay-saying bias)

;

leads to response inconsistency for reversed items;

Measurement:

simultaneous (

dis

)agreement with contradictory statements;

(

dis

)agreement with many heterogeneous items;

net acquiescence as the relative bias away from the midpoint;

different arrangements of the items in the questionnaire should have no differential effect on acquiescent

responding;Slide72

Confirmation bias

when respondents answer a question, they tend to activate beliefs that are consistent with the way in which the item is stated (positive test strategy, inhibition of disconfirming evidence);

this leads to a bias in the direction in which the item is worded (e.g., Are you introverted? vs. Are you extraverted?) and differences in mean response;

the effect of the

keying

direction of the first item on confirmation bias should be strongest in the grouped-massed condition and weakest in the dispersed

condition; Slide73

Example item with 4 negationsTop management in my company has let it be known in no uncertain terms that unethical behaviors will not be tolerated. Slide74

Revised Life Orientation Test (LOT)In uncertain times, I usually expect the best.I’m always optimistic about my future .Overall, I expect more good things to happen to me than

bad.If something can go wrong for me, it will.

I

hardly ever

expect things

to go my

way.

I

rarely count on good things happening

to me.Slide75

Weijters, Baumgartner, and Schillewaert (forthcoming)models in which method effects are included generally yield a much better fit to the data than models in which only substantive factors are included;it is often difficult to clearly distinguish between different method effect specifications on the basis of statistical criteria;

the psychological processes causing method effects are frequently left unspecified;although method factors have been related to a variety of other psychological constructs, the choice of these other constructs often seems

ad hoc

;Slide76
Slide77
Slide78
Slide79

Weijters, Baumgartner, and

Schillewaert

(forthcoming)

models in which method effects are included generally yield a much better fit to the data than models in which only substantive factors are included;

it is often difficult to clearly distinguish between different method effect specifications on the basis of statistical criteria;

the psychological processes causing method effects are frequently left unspecified;

although method factors have been related to a variety of other psychological constructs, the choice of these other constructs often seems

ad hoc

;Slide80

Responses to bogus items

(Meade and Craig, 2012)

%

strongly disagree or disagree responses (1,2)

% other responses

(3-7)

I sleep less than

one hour per night.

90

10

I do

not understand a word of English.

90

10

I have never brushed my teeth.

928I am paid biweekly

by leprechauns.8020

All my friends are aliens.8218

All my friends say I would make a great poodle.7327Slide81

 Strongly Disagree

(1)

Disagree

(

2)

Neither Agree

nor Disagree

(3)

Agree

(

4)

Strongly

Agree

(5)I feel satisfied with the way my body looks right now.

 

 

 

 

 I am satisfied with my weight

 

 

 

 I am pleased with my appearance right now. 

 

 

 

 

I feel attractive

.

 

 

 

 

 

I don’t feel attractive

.

 

 

 

  I feel ugly.     I don’t feel ugly.     Slide82

 Strongly Disagree (1)

Disagree (2)

Neither Agree nor Disagree (3)

Agree (4)

Strongly Agree (5)

A product is more valuable to me if it has some snob appeal

.

 

 

 

 

 

The government should exercise more responsibility for regulating the advertising, sales and marketing activities of manufacturers

.

 

 

 

 

 

Most retailers provide adequate service

.

 

 

 

  

I feel attractive

.

 

 

 

 

 

I don’t feel attractive

.

 

 

 

 

 

I feel ugly

.

     I don’t feel ugly.     Slide83

OPTIM