/
INFLUENCE OF THE ATKINS146 DIET ON INFLUENCE OF THE ATKINS146 DIET ON

INFLUENCE OF THE ATKINS146 DIET ON - PDF document

dandy
dandy . @dandy
Follow
342 views
Uploaded On 2022-08-16

INFLUENCE OF THE ATKINS146 DIET ON - PPT Presentation

115 Chapter 5 BEHAVIO U RAL RESPONSES 116 Influence of the Atkins146 diet on behavio u ral responses Abstract Objective T he effect of the Atkins146s diet was examined over 48 h compared to ID: 936822

146 atkins subjects diet atkins 146 diet subjects meal sleepiness ketosis sleep carbohydrate mood fatigue high control fat dream

Share:

Link:

Embed:

Download Presentation from below link

Download Pdf The PPT/PDF document "INFLUENCE OF THE ATKINS146 DIET ON" is the property of its rightful owner. Permission is granted to download and print the materials on this web site for personal, non-commercial use only, and to display it on your personal computer provided you do not modify the materials and that you retain all copyright notices contained in the materials. By downloading content from our website, you accept the terms of this agreement.


Presentation Transcript

115 Chapter 5 INFLUENCE OF THE ATKINS’ DIET ON BEHAVIO U RAL RESPONSES 116 Influence of the Atkins’ diet on behavio u ral responses Abstract Objective: T he effect of the Atkins’s diet was examined over 48 h compared to a Control mixed diet on sleepiness, mood, fatigue and related symptoms in healthy subjects. Design: This study employed a repeated m easure design where subjects were given isocaloric diets and matching evening test meals , which were either mixed (15% protein, 25% fat, 60% car bohydrate ) or Atkins (38% prote in, 61% fat, 1% carbohydrate). Subjects ’ overall daytime mood, fatigue intensity and sleepiness on the Epworth Sleepiness Scale were assessed before the evening test meals. Symptoms that developed during the Atkins’ diet wer e scored using a modified Atkins’ diet symptom questionnaire. The number of subjects with dream recalls was recorded on awakening after each polysomnographic night Subjects: Fourteen, healthy subjects (18 - 35 y, BMI 20 - 27 kg m - 2 ) underwent a Control diet fol lowed by the Atkins’ diet for 48 h. Measurement: Subject s’ overall daytime mood, fatigue inte

nsity and sleepiness on the Epworth Sleepiness Scale were assessed before the evening test meals. Symptoms that developed during the Atkins’ diet were scored usin g a modified Atkins’ diet symptom questionnaire. The number of subjects with dream recalls was recorded on awakening after each polysomnographic night. Results: Subject s developed mild hypoglycemia and ketosis 48 h following the Atkins’ diet. The daytime symptoms of fatigue, sleepiness and depressed mood were significantly increased following the Atkins’ diet compared to the Control diet. An increased proportion of subjects reporting dreams and cognitive impairments (increasing difficulty with concentratio n and decision making) and other symptoms (craving for sweets, and tired all the time) was also noted 48 h after the Atkins’ diet. Conclusion: The findings suggest that mild hypoglycemia resulting from the diet may mediate the subjective responses of dayt ime sleepiness, depressed mood and intense fatigue. The increased proportion of subjects with dr eam recalls may be related to increased transient arousals from sleep during which dreams may be incorporated into memory. Keywords: Atkins’ diet; Fatigue; Sl eepiness; Mood; Dream

recalls 117 I ntroduction “That what one eats can affect the way one feels and behaves is probably obvious to most laymen.” Dr. Richard Wurtman wrote in his Introduction for the 1982 Conference on Research Strategies for Assessing the B ehavioral Effects of Food and Nutrients (Lieberman & Wurtman 1982) . Today, there is evidence to support the claim that components of food can affect mood, cognition and sleep through biochemical factors acting on the brain. A numbe r of amino acids are known to influence these responses. For an example, L - t ryptophan (Trp ) induce s an earlier sleep onset (Hartmann & Elion 1977; Hartmann & Spinweber 1979; Spinweber et al 1983; Wurtman & Wurtman 19 95) . This s leep behavio u r was also confirmed following consumption of a high glycemic index (GI) carbohydrate meal with a low protein content (Afaghi et al 2007) . The high GI effect on sleep onset is mos t likely mediated by a high insulin response , which promoted uptake of large neutral amino acids (LNAAs) by muscles, giving rise to a high Trp to LNAA ratio ( Trp:LNAA ) , thereby facilitating the entry of T rp into the brain (Fernstron & Wurtman 1971; Layons & Truswell 1988; Berry et al 199

1) . In the brain, TRP is converted to serotonin , which both induces sleep and improves mood (Cole et al 1980) . 118 Previous studies showed that different macronutrients , especially high - fat and high - carbohydrate content of meals confer red a sedative action and promote d sleepiness (Wells et al 1995 ; Wells et al 1998) . High carbohydrate meals were observed to reduce pain perception, induce a state of calmness, sleepiness and satiety . These effects are likely mediated through serotonin following carbohydrate consumption (Young 1991; Wurtman & Wurtman 1995; Zmarzty et al 1997) . High - protein and high - carbohydrate meals with standardized fat content show ed similar postprandial effect s with subjects feeling more feeble, lethargic and mentally slow er after lunch (Wells & Read 1996) . Among st macronutrients , fat similarl y showed a po tent effect on subjective feeling s of sleepiness , boredom, and being feeble after lunch than before eating (Wells et al 1997) , and t h e rating of fatigue was higher 3 h after the meal compared to a high - carbohydrate meal (Wells et al 1997) . A lunch meal containing a high proportio n of fat (50%) , but low content of

carbohydrate induced greater drowsiness, decreased mood, and impaired cognitive performance with a muddled mind compared to a medium - fat medium - carbohydrate lunch (Lloyd et al 1994) . Many of the se responses of sa tiety, analgesia, the state of relaxation (Zmarzty et al 1997) and sleepiness observed after a high fat , high protein and to a much lesser extent high carbohydrate meal are thought to be mediated via 119 cholecystoki nin (CCK) (Wells et al 1997; Zmarzty et al 1997) . However post - lunch sleepiness and fatigue may be due to circadian rhythm affect (Monk 2005 ) and t he timing of meal ingestion al so has a direct influence on postprandial mood and alertness. A high - fat meal compared to a high - carbohydrate meal given in the morning, irrespective of energy content, caused a greater depression in mood and alertness than when given at lunch time (Wells & Read 1996) . T he Atkins’ diet , consisting of 60% fat, 3 0% protein and averaging 10% carbohydrate (Atkins 1992; Kossoff 2004) , is associated with modest weight reduction , but with poor long - term compliance (Westerterp et al 1996; Toubro & Astrup 1997; Brehm et al 2003) . Numerous studies have

shown that restriction of glucose intake elicited carbohydrate craving and suppressed mood (Atkins 1992; Wurtman & Wurtman 1995; Wells et al 1997; Wurtman et al 2003) . Altho ugh an increased daytime sleepiness has been reported following the Atkins’ diet (Atkins 1992) , patients with narcolepsy prescribed the Atkins’ diet demonstrated modest improvements in subjective daytime sleepiness when scored on the Narcolepsy Sym ptom Status Questionnaire, but non significant small changes when scored on the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (Husain et al 2004) . 120 The adverse effects of th e Atkins’ diet include constipation, halitosis, headach e and fatigue (Atkins 1992) . These side effects may be associated with the macronutrients or metabolic by - products of the diet. Although the diet is not prescribed to be hypoenergetic, evidence show s that the diet enhances satiety and reduces overall energy intake (Foster et al 2003) and that reduced c arbohydrate energy metabolism may lead to fatigue. Decreased glucose from normal level and ketosis are evident a few days following commencement of the Atkins diet (Atkins 1992) . Inade quate carbohydrate intake turns on fatty acid m

etabolism to sustain energy supply. Ketones (acetoacetic acid, - hydroxybutyric acid and acetone) that are produced as metabolites of fat oxidation are then released into the blood. Ketone s , through high hepat ic production, provide an alternative metabolic fuel for the brain. As an increased level of ketones passes through the kidneys, some spill into the urine. An i ntake of less than 25 g/d carbohydrate leads to substantial urinary excretion of ketones (Westman et al 2002) . Dr. Atkin s (Atkins 1992) reported th at ketosis and hypoglycemia were factors responsible for the symptoms of “tired all the time” , “ moodiness ” , “ difficu lt with concentration ” , “ poor memory ” , “ bored ” , “ headache ” , “ anger ” , “ sleepiness during day ” and “fatigue”. A systematic 121 documentation of these behavioral changes in a controlled study has not been undertaken . Furthermore, subjects who were on the Atkins’ diet reported anecdotal ly an increased dream tendency. T herefore , in a study with a repeated measure design , healthy subjects were investigated in terms of subjective b

ehavio u ral responses and dream recall frequency after consuming the Atkins diet for 48 h p eriod. Subjects and methods Subjects Fourteen healthy men ( 18 - 35 years, BMI 20 - 27 kgm - 2 ) were recruited from 45 screened volunteers . Subjects were r ecruit ed based on a medical questionnaire and an interview , from the studen t population attending Sydney U niversity . E ach subject signed a consent form that stated the purpose of the study and the nature of the experiment . Subjects were excluded if they had a self - reported current or past history of significant medical, psychiatric or sleep disorders ( nocturnal eating inclusive), used prescribed medication (including sedatives or antidepressants), recreational drugs, or regularly had an alcohol intake of greater than 20 g per day or 100 g per week. Subjects exercising more than three times a week were a lso excluded. During the 48 h prior to the study subjects 122 abstained from any vigorous exercise. Subjects were asked to go to bed at the same time on each of the study nights. They were also asked t o abstain from alcohol for 48 h, and caffeinated beverages for 12 h prior to an

d during the entire testing period spanning 5 nights . Protocol This observation al study on the behavio u ral response s to the Atkins’ diet was part of a stu dy conducted to evaluate objective sleep changes via polysomnography on each t est night . The study consisted of 5 study nights, commencing with a familiarization night, foll owed by a day and evening meal based on a balance d , mixed macronutrient profile ( Control ) and then the Atkins ’ diet for 48 h . The night immediately followed by t he first evening Atkins’ test meal is designated ‘Atkins A cute’ phase , and the night following 48 h on the Atkins’ diet, with an Atkins’ evening test meal is designated ‘Atkins K etosis’ phase ( Table 1 ) . Behavioral d ata were collected for the Control day, t he Atkins A cute and Atkins K etosis days . All evening test meals were eaten 4 h before their usual bedtime. Behavioral data were collected for the control day, the Atkins Acute and Atkins Ketosis days. The measures of ESS (Johns 1991), mood and 1 23 fatigue (B lack et al 2005) were taken before each evening test meal (Table 1) as was the modified Atkins’

; diet - related symptom questionnaire (Atkins 1992). Only sleepiness via the Likert scale (Likert 1932) was measured postprandially at 1, 2, 3 and 4 h (immediately ) prior to bedtime . The number of subjects with dream recall was recorded each morning following waking. Urine was collected for ketone analysis before the evening test meal and at bedtime. Finger - prick blood samples for glucose analysis were taken before (zero time) and after each test meal at 15, 30, 60, 90 and 120 min. Meal S tandard isocaloric (2400 kcal) diets and matching evening test meals ( approximately 1090 k cal ) w ere provided . The Control mixed meal consisted of 16.5% prot ein, 12.5% fat, 71% carb ohydrate , and the Atkins’ meals had a macronutrient distribution of 38% protein, 61 % fat, 1% carbohydrate for the Atkins A cute and Atkins K etosis nights . Ketosis and u rinary ketone level Subjects’ urine was collected on three of the study nights ( Con trol, Atkins A cute and A tkins K etosis) before the evening test meal and at 124 bedtime to monitor urine ketone levels with Multiple Reagent Strips for Urinalysis (Multistix 10 SG Bayer) using the following semi quantitative scale: none, trace

( 0.5 m M ), small ( 1.5 m M ), moderate (4 m M ), large ( ≥ 8 m M ) (Husa in et al 2004) . The ketone level on the evening of the Atkins Ketosis phase served as a check on subjects’ adherence to the Atkins’ diet. A minimum concentration of urinary ketones level of 1.5 mM (Husain et al 2004) had to be reached for subject s to continue in the final sleep study night . Blood Glucose Finger prick bloo d samples for glucose analysis using a glucometer (Medisense Optium TM) were collected at baseline before each test meal and at 15, 30, 45, 60, and 120 min after the meal. Behavio u ral instrume nt Daytime Sleepiness by Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) : Subjects were instructed to choose the most appropriate number on a scale of 0 - 3 for each of the eight situations (see below). Scales included: 0 = would never doze or sleep, 1 = slight chance of do zing or sleeping, 2 = moderate 125 chance of dozing or sleeping, 3 = high chance of dozing or sleeping. The situations were: Sitting and reading, Watching TV, Sitting inactive in a public place, Being a passenger in a motor vehicle for an hour or more, Lying down in the afternoon, Sitting and talking to someone, sitting quietly af

ter lunch (no alcohol), Stopped for a few minutes in traffic while driving. These situations were referred to subject’s usual way of life in recent times. Even if subjects had not don e some of these things recently, they were instructed to work out how they would have affected them. A total score of less than 10 was considered normal, a score of 10 or more was considered sleepy, and a score of 18 or more was very sleepy. The ESS is a r eliable and valid method for measuring daytime sleepiness (Johns 1991) . Subjective r ating of mood, fatigue and postprandial sleepiness (Visual Analogue Scale and Likert scale) : Subjects ranked their overall daily mood for that day using a VAS with a 100mm horizontal line with “ 0 ” being their be st possible overall mood and “ 100 ” being their worst possible overall mood (Black et al 2005) . S ubjects also ranked their daily level of fatigue with “ 0 ” being lack of fatigue and “ 100 ” being highest intensity of fatigue (Black et al 2005) . Subjective rating of post prandial sleepiness wa s assessed after the meal at time points 1, 2, 3, and at 4 h 126 (immediately ) before bedtime by m arking appropri

ately on a four - point sleepiness (Liker scale) , from zero ‘not at all sleepy’ , +1 ‘slightly sleepy’, +2 ‘sleepy’ to +3 ‘very sleepy’. The validity and reliability of this scale has been demonstrated (Hindmarch 1980) . Modified Atkins’ diet - related symptom questionnaire : Th e Atkins’ questionnaire of 49 items that describ ed the symptoms related to ketosis and hypoglycemia were reduced to 27 in the modified questionnaire to include only the most frequent ly reported symptoms during the keto tic period (Atk ins 1992) . Subjects were instructed to use the questionnaire to document the symptom s felt during the day . Dream recall The number of subjects who experienced dreams was recorded the next morning following an overnight polysomnographic testing (sleep data reported elsewhere) . S ubjects were then asked how they felt about their dreams on a three - point scale (1 = pleasant , 2 = neutral, 3 = unpleasant ). 127 Statistics Data were inspected for normality of distribution prior to use of parametric statistics . Data are reported as mean ± standard deviation (SD) . Likert

scale ratings for postprandial sleepiness, VAS for m ood and fatigue intensity , and day time sleepiness (ESS) were analyzed by repeated measures ANOVA and “tests of within - subjects effects ” . Non parametric (K - related samples, Cochrane) test was used to compare the effect of meal types on dream recall. The current protocol has been approved by the Sydney University’s Ethics Committee and consent of the subjects was obtained after the nature of th e procedures had been fully explained. TABLE 1 Study meal plan Day 1 Familiarization 2 Control 3 Atkins’ Acute 4 5 Atkins’ Ketosis Breakfast Mixed meal Mixed meal Mixed meal Atkins’ diet Atkins’ diet Lunch Mixed meal Mixed meal Mixed meal Atkins’ diet Atkins’ diet 6 - 8 h fast on days 2, 3 & 5 Evening meal Mixed meal * Control test meal # Atkins’ test meal Atkins’ diet #Atkins’ test meal Sleep study Sleep study Sleep study Sleep Study * Control test meal: 1090 kcal; 15.5% protein, 12.5% fat and 72% carbohydrate #Atkins’ test meal: 1090 kcal; 38% protein, 61% fat and 1% carbohydrate Daily energy consumption: 2400

kcal. 128 Results Urine ketone s Urine ketone level showed negative traces before the meal and at bedtime for both the C ontrol and Atkins A cute nights (Table 2 ) . This contrasted with an increased ketone level 48 h following commencement of the Atkins ’ diet at Atkins K etosis where 2 subjects showed a low ketone level of 1.5 mM , 6 showed moderate level of 4 mM , and 6 had large level of ≥ 8 mM . TABLE 2 Urinary ketone, subjective ratings of m ood, fatigue, and d ream recalls following a Control meal and Atkins meals ( Acute and Ketosis phase ) a Control m ea l Atkins Acute Atkins Keto sis Overall P P - value c Urine ketone, mM N egative N egative 1.5 - 1 6 Ketosis VAS Mood (mm) 45.8±4.6 b 47.4±4.0 54.1 ± 2.5 P0.001 P0.001 VAS Fatigue (m m) 2.5±1.7 3.2±1.6 29.9±5.0 P0.001 P0.001 * Dream recalls 3 6 8 P=0.09 a n = 14. VAS, visual analogue scale b x ± SD (all such values). c Comparis on of meals between Control and Atkins Acute and Atkins Ketosis *Number of subjects with dream recalls 129 Blood g lucose The baseline blood glucose level before the ev

ening meal for the C ontrol, Atkins A cute and Atkins K etosis phase were 4.28 ± 0.38 , 4.28 ± 0 .43 , and 3.42 ± 0.22 mM respectively. The re was no difference in the baseline blood glucose level between the C ontrol and the Atkins A cute night. However, a significant fall in baseline glucose level was ob served for the Atkins K etosis night ( range 3.1 0 - 3.7 0 mM ) . Following the C ontrol meal, the blood glucose response showed a peak at 45 min, followed by a gradual fall back to baseline level. In contrast, following the Atkins meals at Atkins A cute and Atkins K etosis, a flat glucose response was observed. Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS) The daytime sleepiness score was not significantly different between the Control day (4.2 ± 2.1) and at Atkins Acute (4.4 ± 1.9). At Atkins Ketosis, all participants showed a significant increase in the subjective rating of daytime sleepiness with an ESS score of 11.1 ± 2.4 (P 0.001) ( F igure 1 ). Ten subjects had ESS score of 10 or more and 4 subjects had an ESS score of 8 or 9. 130 Overall mood The subjective r ating s of overall daytime mood between the C ontrol condition (

45.8 ± 4. 6 ) and Atkins A cute condition ( 47.4 ± 4.0) were not significantly different. However, worsened mood was recorded at a rating of 54.1 ± 2. 5 at Atkins K etosis condition (P 0.001 ) (Table 2 ) . 4.4 11.1 4.2 1012141618 Control Atkins Acute Atkins Ketosis Meal ESS score (units) FIGURE 1. Comparison of the sleepiness score using the Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS) between the 3 test meals (Control mixed, Atkins Acute and Atkins Ketosis). Mean ESS score ( - - - ) on Atkins Ketosis was significantly increased compared to Control and Atkins Acute (n = 14, repeated measures ANOVA and “tests of within - subjects effects P 0.001 ). 131 Fatigue intensity The “ Fatigue ” intensity at the Atkins A cute phase ( 3.2 ± 1.6 ) did not c hange significantly from the C ontrol day ( 2. 5 ± 1. 7 ) . A significant increase in rating for this item was recorded at At kins K etosis (29. 9 ± 5.0 , P 0.001) (Table 2 ) . Post prandial Sleepiness scale (Likert scale ) The subjective ratings showed progress ive increase in sleepiness each hour up to bedtime (P 0.001) following the evening test meals for

Control, A tkins Acute an d Atkins Ketosis ( Figure 2 ). Th e interaction o f meal type and time after meal ingestion was not significant (P� 0.1). 0.51.52.5 01234 Time from meal ingestion (h) Likert scale score (units) ........ Control Atkins Acute Atkins Ketosis FIGURE 2. The time course of subjective sleepiness scores over a 4 h pe riod post - test meals to bedtime. n = 14 132 Modified Atkins’ diet - r elated symptom responses : No symptoms were experienced on the Control day or the Atkins Acute day . In contrast, at the Atkins K etosis phase many symptoms were experienced including ‘fatigue’, ‘craving for sweets’, ‘tired all the time’, and ‘sleepiness duri ng the day’ ( Figure 3 ) . Subjective report s of c ogniti ve function included ‘poor motivation’, ‘difficulty with concentration’ and ‘difficulty with decision making’. Negative emotions of boredom , depress ion and ang er were also expressed at the Atkins Ketosis phase . Dream recall There was a trend in increased number of subjects reporting dreams during Atkins Acute and Ketosis phase compared to control (overall P =

0.09) (Table 2). Unpleasant dreams were reported in 50% of those who dreamt during the Atkins Ketosis phase. 133 20406080100120 Fatigue Craving for sweet Tired all the time Sleepiness during day Bored Can't fall sleep easily Slow start in the morning Can't work under pressure Poor motivation Difficulty with concentration Dizziness Difficulty making decision Moodiness Constipation Depressed Headache Waking during night Symptoms Proportion of subjects Control Atkins Ketosis FIGURE 3. Comparison of the proportion of subjects experiencing the Atkins diet - related symptoms between Control and Atkins Ketosis for 14 subjects. Symptom items (17 only) are displayed. Discussion The present study reports the beh avio u ral responses to short - term Atkins’ diet and dream recall frequen cy in healthy adult males. S ubjects did not experience any unusual responses on the Control day or following the first Atkins’ meal (Atkins Acute). Mild hypoglycemia ( 3.42 ± 0.22 mM) 134 and ketosis developed 48 h (Atkins Ketosis) after commencement of the Atkins’ diet. These symptoms coincided with the subjective report of increased daytime sleepiness, increased fatigue intensity, worsened mood and concentration,

and increased number of subj ects with dream recalls of an unpleasant natu re. The symptoms experienced by subjects may implicate origins from a number of factors that act either singly or in combination. The factors of ketosis, hypoglycemia, possible serotonin diminution, and increase d cholescystokinin (CCK) resulting from a high fat, high protein and low carbohydrate diet (Atkins 1992; Wells et al 1997; Wurtman et al 2003) are considered below. The time course of the observed symptom changes at Atkins Ketosis and their coincidence with hypoglycemia and ketosis provide s a feasible link for the latter to explain daytime sleepiness, intense fatigue and depressed mood. However a se arch of the literature, failed to demonstrate the causal relationsh ip between ketosis and these symptoms. In contrast, there is much evidence linking hypoglycemia to these responses. Our observation of hypoglycemia following the Atkins’ diet confirms the previously observed (Atkins 1992) . 135 Subjective report s of daytime sleepiness, fatigue, poor cognition, negative mood related to irritability, anger , a lack of motivation and a craving for sweets during Atkins Ketosis strongly suggest that hypoglycemia

was a primary factor responsible for these sensations. This claim is supported by published literature discussed below. D uring the Atkins Ketosis phase subjects showed a significant increase in the Epworth Sleepiness Scale �10 (Figure 1 ) . This score ind icated an increased sleep propensity which was not influenced by the presence of sleep disorder s or previous sleep deprivation as evidenced by their normal sleep schedules recorded in their sleep diary, and full polysomnograph on the familiarization night. The observed daytime sleepiness in our subjects was a result of the Atkins’ diet , which wa s likely explained by the mild hypoglycemia . A blood glucose concentration of ≤4 mM has prev iously been observed to lead to increase d drowsiness and decrease d alertness that coincided with an increased level of electroencephalographic (EEG) theta (4 - 8 Hz) waves (Cox et al 2000) . Hypoglycemia at a level ≤ 3.6 mM also impaired cognitive functioning (Cox et al 2000) . 136 Although subjects reported an increased daytime sleepiness, the subjective ratings of postprandial sleepi ness indicated ‘not at all sleepy’ or only ‘slightly sleepy’ for t

he first three hours after each of the evening meals (Control, Atkins Acute and Ketosis). It was not until the fourth hour post meal, at their usual bedtime , before they indicate d that they were ‘sleepy’ (Figure 2 ). This increased feeling of sleepiness just before bedtime, however, was not significantly different between the three meals suggesting that factors other than the meals may dictate sleep propensity. All of the meals were served at the same t ime for each subject between 1800 - 20 00 h, that is 4 h befo re the subjects’ usual bedtim e of 2206 - 23 46 h . Th e meal period appears to fall within the ‘forbidden zone’ (sleep propensity is lowest) for sleep (Wells et al 1998) and thus may explain why our subjects did not feel sleepy after the meals regardles s of the meal type, mixed or Atkins’. On the other hand, the subjects were increasingly sleepy close to their usual bedtime (P 0.001, Figure 2 ) suggesting that circadian timing of sleep bears a strong influence on sleepiness (Van den Heuvel et al 1998) . It would be expected that m eals that a re served outside of the ‘forbidden zone’ should induce sleepiness. Subjects fed a hig

h - fat, low - carbohydrate meal in the morning or at lunch time showed suppressed alertness 2 - 3 h after the meal compared to a low - fat, high - carbohydrate meal (Wells et al 1995; Wells et al 1997) . The changes in 137 postprandial sleepiness were thought to be related to cholescystokinin ( CCK ) release ; the timing of its maximum release coincide d with the onset of sleepiness (Wells et al 1997) . It has been shown that CCK is released approximately 2 - 3 h after a high fat , high protein meal , a nd to a much lesser extent after a high carbohydrate meal, but not significantly affected by a mixed meal (Wells et al 1997) . Fifty percent of subjects reported feeling of “moodines s” during Atkins Ketosis (Figure 3 ). S ubjects’ overall mood ratings taken during the day before the evening meal may be related to a diminished level of serotonin resulting from a low blood glucose level and high protein content of the diet . Serotonin is involved in mood control (Young 1991; Wurtman & Wurtman 1995) a nd its level in the brain is dependent on its precurs or Trp . In turn Trp entry into the brain depends on plasma Trp: LNAA . A high ra tio thus facilitates

its entry into the brain, whereas a low ratio resulting from a low carbohydrate - high protein meal similar to that of the Atkins’ meal would impede Trp entry into the brain (Wurtman et al 2003) . Tiredness and fatigue are symptoms of hypoglycaemia (McAulay et al 2001) . At Atkins Ketosis, 98% of subjects experienced ‘feeling of 138 fatigue’ and 79% reported ‘tired all the day’ (Fig ure 3 ). Indeed, many of the symptoms experienced during Atkins Ketosis are tied to mild hypoglycemia, with 79% of subjects reporting a carbohydrate cra ving, 64% ‘slow start in the morning’, 57% ‘poor motivation’, and 57% ‘ difficulty with concen tration’ (Fig ure 3 ). These symptoms , inclusive of sleepiness and hypoglycemia during the short - term Atkins’ diet may have implications for work performance and driving (Frier 2000) . The highest rates of traffic accident victims has been reported in Saudi Arabia, United Arab Emirates , and in a London hospital during the Ramadan fasting period attribut able to reduced alertness (Roky et al 2004) . The blood glucose concentration during Ramadan has been reported as low as 3.7 ± 0.6 mM , which

was significantly lower than compared to before the start of Ramadan 5.2 ± 0.4 m M (P 0.001) (Aybak et al 1996) . Those who had fasted reported an increased irritability (Kaderi et al 2000; Roky et al 2004) similar to subjects in the present study who expressed feelings of ‘anger’ during Atkins Ketosis. Irri tability may compound the incidence of motor vehicle accidents. It was interesting to note that the number of subjects with dream recalls increased with progression of the Atkins’ diet . The highe st proportion of subjects with dream recall s occurred at the Atkins Ketosis phase . These 139 dream recalls may be explained by a higher rate of transient EEG arousal s that occurred during light sleep stages 1 - 2 ( part of a study conducted to evaluate objective sleep changes ) during both Atkins Acute and Atkins Ketosis. The mean arousal (stage 1 - 2) index per hour for the Control night was 11.2 ± 4.2, which was significantly lower than that for the Atkins Acute (14.9 ± 6.0, P = 0.02 ) and Atkins Ketosis (14.2 ± 6.4, P = 0.02 ) nights. Transient arousals from sleep leading to dream fixation has been documented. When awoken dur

ing the periods of increased spontaneous eyelid movements (i.e. EEG arousals) during stage 2 and REM sleep , the experimental subjects reported i magery dreams (Conduit e t al 2004) . It is known that dreams are usually consolidated into memory by the event of awakening itself (Muzur 2005) . Several investigations have confirmed a positive correlation between frequency of dream recall and the frequency of nocturnal awakenings in healthy subjects (Cory et al 1975; Halliday 1988; Schredl & Montass er 1996 - 1997) . We observed a non significant trend in increased total wake time ( P � 0.1, data reported elsewhere) during Atkins Ketosis night compared to the Control and Atkins Acute nights. It is feasible that the wake time during the sleep period may c ontribute to the increased number of dreams being recalled. 140 This is consistent with a higher proportion of subjects recalling dreams during the Atkins Ketosis phase. Dream content most likely reflects waking life stressors (Schredl et al 1998) and the incorporation of stressful elements of wakefulness into dreams (Koulack et al 1985) . In the study self - description of dream emotions which were ch aracterized with themes of “depre

ssion”, “negatives” and “unpleasant” may reflect the physiological stress related to the metabolic process and biochemical changes that occurred following the Atkins’ diet , in particular during Atkins Ketosis with mild hypo glycemia or ketosis. In conclusion we explored behavioral responses to the At kins’ diet on the short term. It is suggest ed that that mild hypoglycemia resulting in from the diet may explain the subjective responses of daytime sleepiness, depressed mood a nd intense fatigue . Following the Atkins diet a greater proportion of the subjects reported dream recalls , particularly during Atkins Ketosis. 141 References Afaghi A, O'Connor H, Chow CM (2007): High - glycemic - index carbohydrate mea ls shorten sleep onset. Am J Clin Nutr 85: 426 - 30. Atkins R (1992): Dr. Atkins new revolution . New York, Avon Books. Aybak M, Turkoglu A, Sermet A, Denli O (1996): Effect of Ramadan fasting on platelet aggregation in healthy male subjects. Eur J Physiol 73 : 552 - 556. Berry EM, Growdon JH, Wurtman JJ, Caballero B, Wurtman RJ (1991): A balanced carbohydrate: protein diet in the management of Parkinson's disease. Neurology 41: 1295

- 97. Black CD, O'Connor PJ, McCully KK (2005): Increased daily physical activity and fatigue symptoms in chronic fatigue syndrome. Dynamic Medicine, http:///www.daynamic - med.com/content 4:3 DOI: 10.1 186/1476 - 5918 - 4 - 3 Brehm BJ, Seeley RJ, Daniels SR, D'Alessio DA (2003): A randomise d trial comparing a very low carbohydrate diet and a calorie - restricted low fat diet on body weight and cardiovascular risk factors in healthy women. J Cin Endocrinol Metab 88(4): 1617 - 1623. Cole J, Hartmann E, Brigham P (1980): L - Tryptophan: clinical stud ies. McLean Hosp J 5: 37 - 71. 142 Conduit R, Crewther SG, Coleman G (2004): Spontaneous eyelid movements (ELMS) during sleep are related to dream recall on awakening. J Sleep Res 13(2): 137 - 44. Cory TL, Orniston DW, Simmel E, Dainoff M (1975): Predicting the fr equency of dream recall. J Abnorm Psychol 84: 261 - 266. Cox DJ, Gonder - Prederick LA, Kovatchev BP, Julian DM, Clark WL (2000): Progressive hypoglycemia's impact on driving simulation performance occurrence, awareness, and corection. Diabetes Care 23: 163 - 17 0. Fernstron JD, Wurtman RJ (1971): Brain serotonin content: increase following ingestion of carbohydrate diet. Science 174: 1023 - 25. Foste

r G, Wyatt H, Hill J (2003): A randomised trial of a low - carbohydrate diet for obesity. N Engl J Med 348: 2082 - 90. Fr ier BM (2000): Hypoglycemia and driving performance. Diabetes Care 23: 148 - 150. Halliday G (1988): Relationship of spontaneous awakenings to dreams and nightmares. Study Dreams Newslett. 5: 4. Hartmann E, Elion R (1977): The insomnia of 'sleeping in a stra nge place': effect of L - Tryptophan. J Psychopharmacol 53: 131 - 133. Hartmann E, Spinweber CL (1979): Sleep induced by L - tryptophan: effect of dosages within the normal dietary intake. J Nerv Ment Dis 167(8): 497 - 99. Hindmarch I (1980): Psychomotor function and psychoactive drugs. Br J Clin Pharmacol 10: 189 - 209. 143 Husain AM, S. YJW, Carwile ST, P. MP, Westman EC (2004): Diet therapy for narcolepsy. Neurology 62: 2300 - 2302. Johns MW (1991): A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness; the Epworth Sleepiness S cale. Sleep 14: 540 - 545. Kaderi N, Tilane A, Batal ME, Taltit Y (2000): Irritability during month of Ramadan. Psychosom Med 62: 280 - 285. Kossoff EH (2004): More fat and fewer seizures: dietary therapies for epilepsy. Lancet Neurol 3: 415 - 420. Koulack D, Pr evost F, De Koninck J (1985): Sleep, dream

ing, and adaptation to a stressful intellectual activity. Sleep 8(3): 244 - 53. Layons PM, Truswell AS (1988): Serotonin precursor influenced by type of carbohydrate meal in healthy adults. Am J Clin Nutr 47: 433 - 39. Lieberman HR, Wurtman RJ (1982): Research strategies for assessing the behavioral effects of foods and nutrients: proceedings of the conference of the center for brain sciences and metabolism charitable trust, , Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Camb ridge, USA, Pergamon Press. Lloyd HM, Green MW, Rogers PJ (1994): Mood and cognitive performance effects of isocaloric lunches differing in fat and carbohydrate content. Physiol Behav 56(1): 51 - 57. McAulay V, Deary IJ, Frier BM (2001): Symptoms of hypoglyc aemia in people with diabetes. Diabet Med 18(9): 690 - 705. 144 Muzur A (2005): Toward an integrative theory of sleep and dreaming. J Theor Biol 233: 103 - 118. Roky R, Houti I, Moussamih S, Qotbi S, Aadil N (2004): Physiological and chronobiological changes durin g ramadan intermittent fasting. Ann Nutr Metab 48: 296 - 303. Schredl M, Montasser A (1996 - 1997): Dream recall: state or trait variable? part 2: states factors, investigations, and final conclusions. Imagination, Cognition and

Personality 16: 231 - 261. Schred l M, Schafer G, Weber B, Heuser I (1998): Dreaming and insomnia: dream recall and dream content of patients with insomnia. J Sleep Res 7: 191 - 198. Spinweber CL, Ursin R, Hilbert RP, Hilderbrand RL (1983): L - tryptophan: effects on daytime sleep latency and the waking EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55(6): 652 - 61. Toubro S, Astrup A (1997): Randomised comparison of diets for maintaing obese subjects' weight after major weight loss: ad lib, low fat, high carbohydrate diet vs. fixed energy intake. Br Med J 314: 29 - 34. Van den Heuvel CJ, Noone JT, Lushington K, Dawson D, van den Heuvel CJ, Noone JT, et al (1998): Changes in sleepiness and body temperature precede nocturnal sleep onset: evidence from a polysomnographic study in young men. J Sleep Res 7(3 ): 159 - 66. Wells AS, Read NW (1996): Influences of fat, energy, and time of day on mood and performance. Physiol Behav 59(6): 1069 - 1079. 145 Wells AS, Read NW, Craig A (1995): Influences of dietary and intraduodenal lipid on alertness, mood, and sustained conc entration. Br J Nutr 74: 115 - 123. Wells AS, Read NW, Idzikowski C, Jones J (1998): Effects of meals on objective and subjective measures of daytime

sleepiness . J Appl Physiol 84(2): 507 - 515. Wells AS, Read NW, Uvnas - Mober g K, Alster P (1997): Influences of fat and carbohydrate on postprandial sleepiness, mood, and hormones. Physiol Behav 61(5): 679 - 86. Westerterp KR, Verboeket VWP, Westerterp PMS, Velthuis TEJ, De Graaf C, Weststrate JA (1996): Dietary fat and body fat: an intervention study. Int J Obesity Related Metab Disord 20: 1022 - 1026. Westman EC, Yancy WS, Edman JS, Tomlin KF, Perkins CE (2002): Effect of 6 - month adherence to a very low carbohydrate diet program. Am J Med 113(1): 30 - 36. Wurtman RJ, Wurtman JJ (1995): Brain serotonin, carbohydrate - craving, obesity and depression. Obes Res 3 suppl 4: 477s - 80s. Wurtman RJ, Wurtman JJ, Regan MM, McDermott JM, Tasay RH, Breu JJ (2003): Effect of normal meals rich in carbohydrates or proteins on plasma tryptophan and tyros ine ratios. Am J Clin Nutr 77: 128 - 32. 146 Young SN (1991): Some effects of dietary components (amino acids, carbohydrate, folic acids) on brain serotonin synthesis, mood, and behavior. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 69: 893 - 903. Zmarzty SA, Wells AS, Read NW (1997): The influence of food on pain perception in healthy human volunteers. Physiol Behav 62(1): 18